Exuberance: The Passion for Life (15 page)

Read Exuberance: The Passion for Life Online

Authors: Kay Redfield Jamison

BOOK: Exuberance: The Passion for Life
5.13Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Extraverts are not only more likely than introverts to experience positive moods, they also feel a
greater intensity in such moods. In responding to questionnaires, they tend to agree with items like “When I feel happy it is a strong kind of exuberance,” “When something good happens, I am usually much more jubilant than others,” and “When I’m happy I bubble over with energy.” Introverts do not. In addition to experiencing more intensely positive moods, extraverts also feel and perform better in stressful and challenging circumstances.

People who are extraverted are more likely to act, to move, to engage. They lope, not amble; they fizz. They are like the infectiously exuberant writer Eugene Walter, who says, “
We are the ones who gallop ahead two hundred miles and then stop and say, ‘What country is this?’ If we could organize, we would have taken over the world way back, but we are interested in so many things that when we head for California, we end up in Florida. You know. Our emblem is the centaur: half animal, half man. And shooting that arrow at the moon. Centaurs have all four feet on the ground, but that arrow is whizzing off to a distant planet.”

Why are some so vital and others not? Why do some people gallop full throttle into adventure while others fall back, fearful, intent on avoiding misadventure? There are many reasons, but the most notable differences in temperament are rooted in genetics and in the architecture and chemistry of the brain. Heredity unequivocably plays a critical role in temperament. Some individuals are simply more biologically predisposed to respond with fear when confronted with a new or uncertain situation; others are inclined to enjoy or investigate the unfamiliar. The genetic contribution to temperament, especially to extraversion, is strong.
An analysis of 24,000 twins found that if one identical twin is an extravert, the other is very likely to be one as well. This is not nearly as true for fraternal (nonidentical) twins. (The correlation between identical twins is 0.5 or more, suggesting that at least 50 percent of extraversion is due to genetic factors. The correlation between fraternal twins is 0.2.)

Thomas Bouchard and his colleagues at the University of Minnesota examined identical twins who had been raised apart and found that if one twin was extraverted, the other twin, who had identical genetic material but a different environment of upbringing, was very likely to be extraverted as well (the correlation was high: 0.6). This gave strong support to the argument that there is a powerful genetic influence on temperament and that family upbringing has less of a determining role. “
Joy, good cheer, and bubbliness,” the Minnesota researchers reported, were particularly heritable in their study.
Studies of young children who are identical twins and who have been classified as having either an inhibited or an uninhibited temperament, analogous to extraverted and introverted temperaments in adults, also show correlations of between 0.5 and 0.6 for these types of temperament. If only those twins classified as extremely inhibited or extremely disinhibited are examined, the correlation is even higher, ranging between 0.7 and 0.9.

We are not so different from domestic dogs in our heritability and differences in temperament.
All breeds of dog show a powerful genetic influence on behavior (again, generally in the range of 0.5). As with humans, there are significant differences in temperament between individual dogs within a breed, but there are also striking differences across breeds. The behavioral traits that most reliably differentiate breeds of dogs are excitability and general activity level, traits related to extraversion in humans. Basset hounds, for example, who would be on few judges’ lists for Most Exuberant in Show, are among the lowest scorers on both activity and excitability; terriers, on the other hand, are highly excitable and propulsively active. Breeds of dogs also vary enormously in the extent to which they demonstrate inhibited or excitable social behaviors, such as body and tail posture, tremor, exploratory and escape behaviors, and changes in heart rate. Some breeds are timid, others more curious or fierce.
Swedish researchers studied behavior in more than 15,000 dogs of 164 species and found a major “personality” factor which consisted of playfulness, the tendency to explore, an interest in the chase, and sociability. This trait, which pervasively influenced the dogs’ behavior, is also observed in seven- to
nine-week-old wolf pups. Confronted with a novel situation, the most fearless pups are also the most playful and exploratory. The shyer dogs, on the other hand, are uninterested in playing, more cautious, and less curious. The most exploratory and fearless pups, when tested a year later, are the most “dominant” within their pack.

There is
little difference in playfulness between male and female dogs; studies of human children, on the other hand, find
boys are more likely than girls to be physically spontaneous, to explore more and to explore larger areas, to engage in “rough-and-tumble” play, and to be less socially inhibited. Studies of adult humans also find that
men are more likely than women to be energetic and to be perceived by others as enthusiastic and unrestrained.

Our species, like others, shows a wide diversity of temperaments: some of us rush toward the new and assume that it will bring pleasure, not wretchedness. As many at least step back from life, stay within a sprint of our foxholes, and watch as others make the forward moves. The disposition to advance or to retreat, to be enthusiastic or to be fearful, shows itself early in life.
In a landmark series of studies, Jerome Kagan and his colleagues at Harvard identified temperament profiles in infants and young children that are strikingly similar to those seen in adults. Behaviorally inhibited infants and children, like introverted adults, will, when confronted with new people or new situations, actively avoid engagement with the unfamiliar. If they do approach, they do so slowly and reluctantly. They cling to their parents, are quieter, and move less energetically than their more extraverted peers.

Uninhibited children, on the other hand, readily initiate contacts with others, laugh and smile a lot, and are unusually talkative. They are extraverts in the making. A subgroup, perhaps one in ten of those studied by Kagan and his coworkers, displays an unusually high level of energy, smiles frequently, and laughs with “zeal.” This quality, the researchers conclude, is “
difficult to name, for it is not captured simply by activity level. Other children run a lot but do not possess the enthusiasm and vibrancy that is distinctive of these children. The term
vitality
comes closest.” The Harvard scientists found that this characteristic manifests itself at a remarkably young age. At four months, the infants who show a vibrant and positive mood also babble and smile a lot, exhibit little or no anxiety, and are “utterly fearless.” When examined more than ten years later, these high-mood and high-energy infants—whom Kagan calls “Ethel Merman types”—are very likely to have remained extraverted and energetic.

Uninhibited children, in addition to being more exuberant, are far more likely to take risks than those who are inhibited. In a study
of five-and-a-half-year-olds, for example, children were asked to choose the distance from which they preferred to throw a ball into a basket. The inhibited children opted to stand very close to the basket, only one or two feet away. The uninhibited children, on the other hand, more often chose to stand four or five feet back. The investigators noted that these children looked as though they enjoyed the greater challenge.

Nathan Fox, of the University of Maryland, describes a group of highly energetic and enthusiastic infants whom he and his colleagues have studied.
Characterized by the researchers as “exuberant,” these infants are highly and eagerly reactive to novelty; they smile, coo, and gurgle a great deal, are sociable and enthusiastic, and seem exceptionally eager to explore the world around them. They show little fear in unfamiliar situations. “From the youngest age,” the researchers observe, “these infants appeared to exhibit exuberance for novelty and social interaction that was unique.” Their description of the moods and behaviors of these exuberant infants was strongly confirmed by the observations made by the children’s parents. Fox and his colleagues found, as Kagan did, that the “exuberant” infants made up about 10 percent of those they studied.

The psychologist Ellen Winner, who studies artistically and intellectually gifted children, states that even in infancy exuberant children show very high energy levels and are unusually alert and curious. “
From an early age,” she writes, “these children find things that interest them and they throw themselves into these domains.” One eight-year-old child she studied “created hundreds of soldiers, each wearing a uniform of a particular country and rank (which he learned about by reading). He made these soldiers out of paper. When he was finished with this project, he created hundreds (literally) of zoo animals, each one very realistic, and designed cages for them.” She describes his behavior as exuberant,
she says, because “he became completely immersed in this activity for months, it required high energy, and it clearly gave him pleasure. He lived in his own private world.”

Many studies find a strong positive relationship between curiosity, a preference for novelty, and subsequent measures of intelligence and academic performance.
Infants who gaze more at novel objects, for example, score higher on later tests of cognitive ability. (
Likewise in our primate cousins. Curiosity predicts successful problem-solving in small-eared bushbabies.) Shy and anxious children, who tend to avoid novel objects or situations, perform less well on tasks measuring creativity. Seeking out novelty shows itself early. Researchers at the University of Southern California tested the tendency to seek out stimulation in 1,795 three-year-olds and then assessed their cognitive abilities when they were eleven.
Children who scored high on stimulation-seeking when young—defined by the researchers as greater exploratory behavior away from their mothers, more friendliness and talking to strangers, and more active social play—scored significantly higher on IQ tests (12 points). They also had higher scholastic and reading ability than the children who had scored lower on stimulation-seeking.

There are several possible explanations for this unusually strong correlation (0.5 to 0.9) between an active, curious temperament and cognitive ability. Young children who are shy or anxious may so fear criticism from others and be so desirous of pleasing that they do not take the risks necessary to enhance their lives of play and imagination. These children, often more afraid of failing than excited by the chance of winning, put such a premium on “getting it right” that they limit their exploration of the field of possibilities. Young children who seek out novelty or actively explore the world around them, on the other hand, create a very much enriched environment in which to learn. They are probably innately more highly energetic and motivated, as well. Research
indicates that
curious, enthusiastic, and cheerful children also have a more positive effect on their parents, as well as on other children, teachers, and other adults. This in turn positively affects the child’s overall social and learning environment. Exuberant children create more complex and rewarding environments for themselves than do shyer, more timid children.

The interaction between environment and temperament is inordinately complex, of course, and it only gets more complex the more it is studied. All animals are shaped not only by their genes, but by the circumstances under which they develop; as the science writer Matt Ridley has put it, “
Nature versus nurture is dead. Long live nature via nurture.”
Mice and rats, we know, grow more neurons in the hippocampus when they are brought up in an enriched environment rather than in a standard laboratory cage. Rat pups removed from their nests and given more freedom to explore for a few minutes every day for the first three weeks of life show less anxiety and more exploratory behavior later.
Rhesus monkey infants raised in an enriched environment, with a terrycloth-covered movable surrogate monkey and a terrycloth diaper covering a “water bed”—rather than in a standard environment, with a stationary surrogate and only a terrycloth diaper—do much better when they are tested later on a variety of problem-solving and motor tests. There is, as well, an interaction between an animal’s innate temperament and how it is raised. Those monkeys who do the best on subsequent testing are those who rate low on fearfulness during their first month of life and who are also reared in an enhanced environment. Those who do worst, on the other hand, are those who display a fearful temperament when very young and have no exposure to an enhanced environment.

Strong temperamental differences in young nonhuman as well as human infants, together with findings from studies of identical and nonidentical twins demonstrating a strong genetic influence on
temperament, make it clear that biological factors are importantly involved in exuberant behavior. One of the most consistently implicated of these biological factors is dopamine, a neurotransmitter of ancient origins. Dopamine, one of many neurotransmitters in the brain responsible for the regulation of moods and motivated behaviors (others include, importantly, serotonin and norepinephrine), long predates our earliest mammalian ancestors; indeed, its existence can be traced back to mollusks and other invertebrates that lived more than half a billion years ago. Dopamine does many things: it heightens attention to unexpected happenings in the environment; it regulates movement; and, most critically here, dopamine is central to reward. Dopamine is released in the brain in response to behaviors of obvious evolutionary value such as obtaining food, drink, and sex. But the plunge downward on a roller coaster also increases dopamine, as does a win at gambling or a line of cocaine. Music, too, as we shall see, has a powerful effect.
Brain imaging studies conducted while a person is listening to music show that there are increases in cerebral blood flow in the same reward areas of the brain that are active when food, sex, or highly addictive drugs are involved. (Music may also, like other inducers of positive mood, decrease activity in those regions of the brain associated with negative emotions, such as anxiety or revulsion.)

Other books

True Blend by DeMaio, Joanne
Just a Queen by Jane Caro
Ribbons by Evans, J R
Rising Sun: A Novel by Michael Crichton
Descent Into Chaos by Ahmed Rashid
Whitehorse by Katherine Sutcliffe
Second Chance by Angela Verdenius
Fire by C.C. Humphreys