Read Of Minds and Language Online

Authors: Pello Juan; Salaburu Massimo; Uriagereka Piattelli-Palmarini

Of Minds and Language (73 page)

BOOK: Of Minds and Language
13.99Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Finally, this is a more technical remark: E-language versus I-language. We had this discussion with Janet Fodor. Ever since Noam insisted on this distinction (it was systematized in
Knowledge of Language
(1986), but it was there all the time), it has been interesting to see how much work has been done in the context of E-language, languages that allegedly are “out there.” The best example of this, which I'm not recommending, is Terrence Deacon's book (1997) on the evolution of language, in which brains are evolving and languages are also evolving and they supposedly evolve together. For Deacon languages are things out there and his idea is that our brain had been evolving with those languages out there. This is not the way to think about that; it is the computational state that we have here inside. A lot of work, notably all the learnability work – Gold's theorem and all the linguistic strings coming in, as Janet Fodor has aptly reminded us here (see
Chapter 17
) – has interesting aspects but the challenge is to translate those things so that they still make sense in an I-language context. Martin Nowak and other mathematicians (Nowak et al 2001a, b, 2002) have been publishing widely on the evolution of
language but it's entirely on E-language evolving and some of that work may have some interest if translated into I-language. This change from E-language to I-language has been very important and, as Jerry Fodor insightfully likes to stress, we are forever bewildered by the consequences of what we know. It takes years to understand a radical change like this and all the consequences it entails, and we are looking forward to the years to come dealing with these problems.

P
ELLO SALABURU
: I would like to take just a minute to thank all the participants here, especially Noam, because as I said the first day, it was very difficult for him to come here. So thank you, Noam, we are very, very grateful to you. I would also like to thank the other speakers and all the public who have attended the conference. I think it was a great opportunity for all of us to listen and learn. Finally, I would like to express my deep appreciation to each and every one of the participants.

Noam was telling us yesterday that it takes 25 percent longer to translate words from English into Spanish. You know that we also have a lot of redundancies in Basque too, but at this particular point, when we say “thank you very much” in Basque, we use only two words to do it:
eskerrik asko
. But unfortunately, the number of syllables is the same.

Thank you very much.

References
*

Abzhanov, A., Kuo, W. P., Hartmann, C., Grant, B. R., Grant, P. R., and Tabin, C. J. (2006). The Calmodulin Pathway and Evolution of Elongated Beak Morphology in Darwin's Finches.
Nature
, 442(7102): 563–567.

Agrawal, A., Eastman, Q. M., and Schatz, D. G. (1998). Implications of Transposition Mediated by V(d)j-recombination Proteins Rag1 and Rag2 for Origins of Antigen-specific Immunity.
Nature
, 394: 744–751.

Allis, C. D., Jenuwein, T., Reinberg, D., and Caparros, M.-L. (2006).
Epigenetics
. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, Cold Spring Harbor.

Annett, M. (1998). Handedness and Cerebral Dominance: the Right Shift Theory.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
, 10(4): 459–469.

Appel, T. A. (1987).
The Cuvier–Geoffroy Debate: French Biology in the Decades Before Darwin
. Oxford University Press, USA.

Arthur, W. (2004).
Biased Embryos and Evolution
. Cambridge University Press.

Asher, N. and Morreau, M. (1995). What Some Generic Sentences Mean. In Carlson, G. N. and Pelletier, F. J., editors,
The Generic Book
. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. pp. 300–338.

Bach, E., Jelinek, E., Kratzer, A., and Partee, B. H. (1995).
Quantification in Natural Languages
. Kluwer, Dordrecht, Holland.

Baggaley, J. W. (1999). The Ship of Theseus (by John Baggaley May 10, 1999). http://www.baggaley.org/Baggaley/Essays/Entries/1999/5/10_The_Ship_of_Theseus.html

Bahlmann, J., Gunter, T. C., and Friederici, A. D. (2006). Hierarchical and Linear Sequence Processing: an Electrophysiological Exploration of Two Different Grammar Types.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
, 18(11): 1829.

Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R., and Friederici, A. D. (in press). Hierarchical Sequencing Engages Broca's Area.
NeuroImage
.

Baker, M. C. (2001).
The Atoms of Language: the Mind's Hidden Rules of Grammar
. Basic Books, New York.

—— (2003). Linguistic Differences and Language Design.
Trends in Cognitive Science
, 7(8): 349–353.

Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants' Contribution to the Achievement of Joint Reference.
Child Development
, 62(5): 875–890.

Barner, D. and Snedeker, J. (2005). Quantity Judgments and Individuation: Evidence That Mass Nouns Count.
Cognition
, 97: 41–66.

—— Thalwitz, D., Wood, J., and Carey, S. (2005). Overcoming the Set-size Limitation on Parallel Individuation: Plural Morphology As a Possible Source of “More Than One”. Ms.

Barner, D. Thalwitz, D., Wood, J., and Carey, S. (2007). On the Relation Between the Acquisition of Singular–Plural Morpho-syntax and the Conceptual Distinction Between One and More Than One.
Developmental Science
, 10(3): 365–373.

Barner, D., Wood, J., Hauser, M., and Carey, S. (2008). Evidence for a Non-linguistic Distinction Between Singular and Plural Sets in Rhesus Monkeys.
Cognition
, 107: 603–627.

Bates, E. and MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, Variation, and Language Learning. In MacWhinney, B., editor,
Mechanisms of Language Acquisition
. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. pp. 157–194.

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., and Damasio, H. (1994). Insensitivity to Future Consequences Following Damage to Human Prefrontal Cortex.
Cognition
, 50: 7–15.

Bednekoff, P. A. and Balda, R. (1996). Social Caching and Observational Spatial Memory in Pinyon Jays.
Behaviour
, 133: 807–826.

Bejan, A. and Marden, J. H. (2006). Unifying Constructal Theory for Scale Effects in Running, Swimming and Flying.
The Journal of Experimental Biology
, 209: 238–248.

Belletti, A., editor (2004).
Structures and Beyond. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures
. Oxford University Press, New York.

Bever, T. G. (1970). The Cognitive Basis for Linguistics Structures. In Hayes, J. R., editor,
Cognition and the Development of Language
. J. Wiley and Sons, New York. pp. 279–362.

—— (1975). Cerebral Asymmetries in Humans Are Due to the Differentiation of Two Incompatible Processes: Holistic and Analytic.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
, 163: 251–262.

—— (1980). Broca and Lashley Were Right: Cerebral Dominance is an Accident of Growth. In Kaplan, D. and Chomsky, N., editors,
Biology and Language
. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. pp. 186–230.

—— (1981). Normal Acquisition Processes Explain the Critical Period for Language Learning. Diller, K. C., editor, Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Newbury House Publishers, Inc., Rowley, MA. pp. 176–198.

—— (1987). The Aesthetic Basis for Cognitive Structures. In Brand, W. and Harrison, R., editors,
The Representation of Knowledge and Belief
. University of Arizona Press. pp. 314–356.

—— and Chiarello, R. J. (1974). Cerebral Dominance in Musicians and Nonmu-sicians.
Science
, 185(4150): 537–539.

—— and Hansen, R. E. (1988). The Induction of Mental Structures While Learning to Use Symbolic Systems. In
Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Meeting of The Cognitive Science Society
. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., Hillsdale, NJ. pp. 132–138.

—— and McElree, B. (1988). Empty Categories Access Their Antecedents During Comprehension.
Linguistic Inquiry
, 19(1): 35–43.

—— and Sanz, M. (1997). Empty Categories Access Their Antecedents During Comprehension: Unaccusatives in Spanish.
Linguistic Inquiry
, 28(1): 69–91.

—— Carrithers, C., and Townsend, D. J. (1987). A Tale of Two Brains; Or, the Sinistral Quasimodularity of Language.
Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Cognitive Science Society Meetings
.

—— —— Cowart, W., and Townsend, D. J. (1989a). Language Processing and Familial Handedness. In Galaburda, A., editor,
From Reading to Neurons: Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition
. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. pp. 331–357.

—— Straub, K., Shenkman, K., Kim, J. J., and Carrithers, C. (1990). The Psychological Reality of NP-trace. In Carter, J., Dechaine, R.-M., Philip, B., and Sherer, T., editors,
Proceedings of NELS 20
.

Blais, A. L. (1997).
On the Plurality of Actual Worlds
. University of Massachusetts Press.

Blasco Aznar, P. L. (2002). The Truth of the I and Its Intuitive Knowledge.
Analecta Husserliana
, 76: 117–131.

Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., Hood, L., Bowerman, M., Maratsos, M., and Maratsos, M. P. (1975). Structure and Variation in Child Language.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
, 40(2): 1–97.

Bloom, P. (2002). Mindreading, Communication and the Learning of Names for Things.
Mind and Language
, 17(1 & 2): 37–54.

Bloomfield, L. (1933).
Language
(edited by Charles F. Hockett). Chicago University Press, Chicago.

—— (1939). Etudes phonologiques dédiées a la mémoire de N. S. Trubetzkoy.
Travaux du Cercle Linguistique de Prague
, 8: 105–115.

—— (1987). Menomini Morphophonemics. In Hockett, C. F., editor,
Leonard Bloomfield Anthology
. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. pp. 243–254.

Boden, M. A. (2006).
Mind As Machine: A History of Cognitive Science
. Oxford University Press, Oxford and New York.

Boeckx, C. (2006).
Linguistic Minimalism: Origins, Concepts, Methods, and Aims
. Oxford University Press, USA.

—— (2008).
Understanding Minimalist Syntax
. Blackwell, Oxford.

Bolhuis, J. and Gahr, M. (2006). Neural Mechanisms of Birdsong Memory.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
, 7: 347–357.

Brannon, E. M. and Roitman, J. (2003). Nonverbal Representations of Time and Number in Non-human Animals and Human Infants. In Meck, W. H., editor,
Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing
. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL. pp. 143–182.

—— Wusthoff, C. J., Gallistel, C. R., and Gibbon, J. (2001). Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale.
Psychol Sci
, 12(3): 238–43.

Breitenstein, C., Jansen, A., Deppe, M., Foerster, A. F., Sommer, J., Wolbers, T., and Knecht, S. (2005). Hippocampus Activity Differentiates Good from Poor Learners of a Novel Lexicon.
Neuroimage
, 25(3): 958–968.

Brent, M. R. (1997). Advances in the Computational Study of Language Acquisition. Brent, M. R., editor,
Computational Approaches to Language Acquisition
. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

Bresnan, J. (2000) Optimality Theory. In Dekkers, J., van der Leeuw, F., and van de Weijer, J., editors,
Optimality Theory: Phonology, Syntax, and Acquisition
. Oxford University Press.

—— and Grimshaw, J. (1978). The Syntax of Free Relatives in English.
Linguistic Inquiry Amherst, Mass
., 9(3): 331–391.

Bretherton, I. and Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking About Internal States: the Acquisition of an Explicit Theory of Mind.
Developmental Psychology
, 18(6): 906–921.

Broca, P. (1865). Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articule.
Bulletin de la Société d'anthropologie
, 6: 337–393.

Brodmann, K. (1909).
Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre Der Grosshirnrinde in Ihren Prinzipien Dargestellt Auf Grund Des Zellenbaues
. Barth, Leipzig. [English translation by Laurence Garay 1994 “Brodmann's ‘Localization in the Cerebral Cortex' ”, London, Smith-Gordon. Republished 2005 Springer Verlag.)

Brugos, A., Micciulla, L., and Smith, C., editors (2004).
Proceedings of the 28th Boston University Conference on Language Development
, volume 2.

Bussey, T. J., and Dickinson, A. (2003). Can Animals Recall the Past and Plan for the Future?
Nat Rev Neurosci
, 4(8): 685–691.

—— Emery, N. J., and Dickinson, A. (2006). The Prospective Cognition of Food Caching and Recovery by Western Scrub Jays (Aphelocoma Californica).
Comp Cogn Behav Rev
, 1:1–11.

Camps, M. and Uriagereka, J. (2006). The Gordian Knot of Linguistic Fossils. Ms.

Carey, S. (1985).
Conceptual Change in Childhood
. Bradford Books/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

—— (2001). On the Very Possibility of Discontinuities in Conceptual Development. In Dupoux, E., editor,
Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honour of Jacques Mehler
. A Bradford Book/The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. pp. 303–324.

Carroll, S. B. (2005).
Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo
. Norton, New York.

Cartwright, T. A. and Brent, M. R. (1997). Syntactic Categorization in Early Language Acquisition: Formalizing the Role of Distributional Analysis.
Cognition
, 63(2): 121–170.

Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L., and Weir, J. (1995). A Cross-linguistic Study of Early Lexical Development.
Cognitive Development
, 10(2): 159–199.

Chametzky, R. A. (2003). Phrase Structure. In Hendrick, editor,
Minimalist Syntax
. Blackwell, Oxford. pp. 192–225.

Chan, S., Ryan, L., and Bever, T. G. (in preparation). Only Right Handers from Left-handed Families Have Bilateral Representation for Words, but All People Have Left Hemisphere Dominance for Syntactic Processing.

BOOK: Of Minds and Language
13.99Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Den of Sorrows by Quinn Loftis
Victorious by M.S. Force
Every Boy's Got One by Meg Cabot
Apple Tree Yard by Louise Doughty
The Wet Nurse's Tale by Erica Eisdorfer
Self Condemned by Lewis, Wyndham
Yakima Nights by Archie Kennedy
Las puertas de Thorbardin by Dan Parkinson
Shadow Cave by Angie West