The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ (3 page)

Read The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ Online

Authors: David Shenk

Tags: #Psychology, #Cognitive Psychology & Cognition, #Cognitive Psychology

BOOK: The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ
13.58Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
Think of your own genetic makeup
as the hand of cards you were dealt at conception. With each conception in a family comes a new shuffling of the deck and a new hand. That’s partly why little Bobby sleeps through the night as a baby, always behaves and seems to love math, while brother Billy is colicky, never listens and already is the head of a gang in kindergarten.

Genes dictate. Genes instruct. Genes determine
. For more than a century, this has been the widely accepted explanation of how each of us becomes us. In his famous pea-plant experiments of the 1850s and ’60s,
Gregor Mendel demonstrated that basic traits
like seed shape and flower color were reliably passed from one generation to the next through dominant and recessive “heritable factors” (Mendel’s phrase before the word “gene” was introduced). After eight years and twenty-eight thousand plants,
Mendel had proved the existence of genes—and seemed to prove that genes alone determined the essence of who we are
. Such was the unequivocal interpretation of early-twentieth-century geneticists.

That notion is with us still. “Genes set the stage,” affirms
USA Today
. The environment has an impact on all of our lives, to be sure, but genes come first; they set specific lower and upper limits of each person’s potential abilities.
Where did your brother get that amazing singing voice? How did you get so tall? Why can’t I dance? How is she so quick with numbers?

“It’s in the genes,” we say
.

That’s what
The Bell Curve
authors thought, too. None of these writers realized that
over the last two decades Mendel’s ideas have been thoroughly upgraded—so much so that one large group of scientists now suggests that we need to wipe the slate clean and construct an entirely new understanding of genes
.

This new vanguard is a loose-knit group of geneticists, neuroscientists, cognitive psychologists, and others, some of whom call themselves developmental systems theorists. I call them
interactionists
because of their emphasis on the dynamic interaction between genes and the environment.
Not all of the interactionists’ views have yet been fully accepted
, and they freely acknowledge their ongoing struggle to articulate the full implications of their findings. But it already seems very clear that these implications are far-reaching and paradigm-shifting.

To understand interactionism, you must first try to forget everything you think you know about heredity.
“The popular conception of the gene as a simple causal agent is not valid,” declare geneticists Eva Jablonka and Marion Lamb
. “The gene cannot be seen as an autonomous unit—as a particular stretch of DNA which always produces the same effect. Whether or not a length of DNA produces anything, what it produces, and where and when it produces it may depend on other DNA sequences and on the environment.”

Though Mendel couldn’t detect it with his perfectly calibrated pea-plant hybrids, genes are not like robot actors who always say the same lines in the exact same way. It turns out that they interact with their surroundings and can say different things depending on whom they are talking to.

This obliterates the long-standing metaphor of genes as blueprints with elaborate predesigned instructions for eye color, thumb size, mathematical quickness, musical sensitivity, etc
. Now we can come up with a more accurate metaphor. Rather than finished blueprints,
genes—all twenty-two thousand of them
1
—are more like volume knobs and switches. Think of a giant control board inside every cell of your body.

Many of those knobs and switches can be turned up/down/on/off at any time—by another gene or by any minuscule environmental input
. This flipping and turning takes place constantly. It begins the moment a child is conceived and doesn’t stop until she takes her last breath. Rather than giving us hardwired instructions on how a trait must be expressed,
this process of gene-environment interaction drives a unique developmental path for every unique individual
.

The new interactionists call it “GxE” for short. It has become central to the understanding of all genetics. Recognition of GxE means that we now realize that genes powerfully influence the formation of all traits, from eye color to intelligence, but rarely dictate precisely what those traits will be. From the moment of conception, genes constantly respond to, and interact with, a wide range of internal and external stimuli—nutrition, hormones, sensory input, physical and intellectual activity, and other genes—to produce a unique, custom-tailored human machine for each person’s unique circumstance. Genes matter, and genetic differences will result in trait differences, but in the final analysis, each of us is a dynamic system, a creature of development.

This new dynamic model of GxE (genes multiplied by environment) is very different from the old static model of G+E (genes plus environment). Under the old paradigm, genes came first and set the stage. They dealt each of us our first hand of cards, and only afterward could we add in environmental influences.

The new model begins with interaction. There is no genetic foundation that gets laid before the environment enters in; rather, genes express themselves strictly in accordance with their environment. Everything that we are, from the first moment of conception, is a result of this process. We do not
inherit
traits directly from our genes. Instead, we
develop
traits through the dynamic process of gene-environment interaction. In the GxE world, genetic differences still matter enormously. But, on their own, they don’t determine who we are.

In fact, you did not even inherit your blue eyes or brown hair from your parents’ genes. Not directly.

This may sound crazy at first, because of how thoroughly we’ve been indoctrinated with Mendelian genetics
. The reality turns out to be much more complicated—even for pea plants.
Many scientists have understood this much more complicated truth for years but have had trouble explaining it to the general public
. It is indeed a lot harder to explain than simple genetic determinism.

To understand genes more fully, we first need to take a step back and explain what they actually do:

Genes direct the production of proteins
.

Each of our cells contains a complete double strand of DNA, which in turn contains thousands of individual genes. Each gene initiates the process of assembling amino acids into proteins.
Proteins are large, specialized molecules that help create cells, transport vital elements, and produce necessary chemical reactions
. There are many different protein types, and they provide the building blocks of everything from muscle fiber to eyeball collagen to hemoglobin. We are, each one of us, the sum of our proteins.

Genes contain the instructions for the formation of those proteins, and they direct the protein-building process (Diagram A).

But …
genes are
not
the only things influencing protein construction. It turns out that the genetic instructions themselves are influenced by other inputs. Genes are constantly activated and deactivated by environmental stimuli, nutrition, hormones, nerve impulses, and other genes (Diagram B).

Courtesy of Hadel Studio

This explains how every brain cell and hair cell and heart cell in your body can contain
all
of your DNA but still perform very specialized functions
. It also explains how a tiny bit of genetic diversity goes a very long way: human beings are distinct from one another not just because of our relatively few genetic differences, but also because every moment of our ongoing lives actively influences our own genetic expression.

Think of GxE as baking a cake, suggests Cambridge University biologist Patrick Bateson. A hundred cooks may start out with nearly the same ingredients but will in the end produce very different cakes. While the slight difference in ingredients guarantees that differences will exist, it doesn’t dictate what those differences will be. The actual end-result differences arise out of the process.
“Development is chemistry
,” says Bateson, “and the end product cannot simply be reduced to its ingredients.”

Similarly, the mere presence of a certain gene does not automatically produce a specific type or number of proteins. First, every gene has to be activated—switched on, or “expressed”—in order to initiate protein construction.

Courtesy of Hadel Studio

Further, geneticists have recently discovered that some genes—we don’t yet know how many—are versatile. In some cases, the exact same gene can produce different proteins depending on how and when it is activated.

All of this means that, on their own, most genes cannot be counted on to directly produce specific traits
. They are active participants in the developmental process and are built for flexibility. Anyone seeking to describe them as passive instruction manuals is actually minimizing the beauty and power of the genetic design.

So why do I have brown eyes like my mom and red hair like my dad?

In practical terms, there are many elementary physical traits like eye, hair, and skin color where the process is near Mendelian—where certain genes produce predictable outcomes most of the time. But looks can be deceiving; a simple Mendel-like result doesn’t mean that there wasn’t gene-environment interaction.
“Even in the case of eye color,” says Patrick Bateson, “the notion that the relevant gene is
the
[only] cause is misconceived, because [of] all the other genetic and environmental ingredients
.” Indeed, Victor McKusick, the Johns Hopkins geneticist widely regarded as the father of clinical medical genetics, reminds us that in some instances “two blue-eyed parents can produce children with brown eyes.” Recessive genes cannot explain such an event; gene-environment interaction can.

Other books

Jay Walking by Tracy Krimmer
Stolen Chances by Elisabeth Naughton
Protecting Tricia by Pamela Tyner
Elena by Thomas H. Cook
Troy's Surrender by K.M. Mahoney
Ai by admin;Earl Veneris
Ambitious by Monica McKayhan
By His Majesty's Grace by Jennifer Blake