Read Welcome to Your Brain Online
Authors: Sam Wang,Sandra Aamodt
Tags: #Neurophysiology-Popular works., #Brain-Popular works
guaranteed) that there’s some relationship between them—but you still can’t tell what the
relationship is. For example, knowing that married people are happier on average than
single people doesn’t tell us whether your son would be happier if he got married, no
matter what you might personally believe. Being married could make people happier, or
being happy may simply make it easier to get married. Indeed, psychologists who measure
happiness in the same individuals through several years of their lives have found that both
these statements are true. Happier people are more likely to get married; that, in turn, makes
them happier still. Not all happiness research is correlational, but when we interpret
studies of this type, we need to remember that a correlation between two things can’t tell us
what we would most want to know: which thing causes the other, or if there is a third,
unknown cause of both things.
Another point to keep in mind is that, as with most psychological research, the answer
you get depends greatly on how you ask the question. For instance, when women were
asked to list the activities that they particularly enjoyed overall, “spending time with my
kids” topped the list. In contrast, when other researchers asked women to describe how
they felt during each of their activities the previous day, the average positive rating given to
interacting with children indicated that this activity is roughly as rewarding as doing
housework or answering e-mail. This finding suggests that women find their children more
rewarding in theory than in practice, at least on a moment-to-moment basis.
In its strongest form, the adaptation idea suggests that all efforts to increase happiness in an
individual or society are futile and that people’s life circumstances have no long-term influence on
their happiness. This would be pretty surprising and almost certainly isn’t correct. Indeed, some
circumstances are reliably associated with unhappiness, including chronic pain or having to commute
a long way to work.
The life events most likely to have a lasting negative influence on people’s happiness include the
death of a spouse, divorce, disability, and unemployment. In all these circumstances, people still
adapt—their happiness is much more strongly affected right after the event and then moves back
toward the baseline—but the adaptation is not complete. Even eight years after the death of a spouse,
surviving partners remain less happy than they were when their spouse was alive. Deliberate attempts
to increase happiness have also had some lasting success, though these interventions seem to be most
effective if they are repeated frequently (see
Practical tip: How to increase your happiness
).
When psychologists follow the same people over time, most of them report fairly stable
happiness. In one study of Germans over a seventeen-year period, the happiness of only 24 percent of
the participants changed significantly from the start to the end of the study, and only 9 percent changed
a lot. All individual circumstances—marriage, health, income, and so on—taken together account for
only 20 percent of the differences in happiness from one individual to another in the U.S., while
genetic factors account for about 50 percent of the differences. Identical twins reared apart (usually
because they were adopted separately) are much more similar to each other in their adult happiness
than fraternal twins who are reared apart, and about as similar in happiness as identical twins reared
together. (The mysterious remaining 30 percent includes measurement errors, such as the differences
between individuals in defining survey responses like “mostly satisfied.”)
In general, the brain seems to respond more strongly to changes than to persistent conditions, right
down to the level of single cells. Neurons also show adaptation (though they typically do it in less
than a second, not months). Adaptation is efficient because most of the information in the world is
stable, while most of the action that is important to your brain lies in the part of the world that is
changing—objects that are moving, your mate’s new facial expression, or an unexpected source of
food. If the brain can cheat by devoting its limited resources to representing the information that is
new, it may be able to more effectively help you respond to the world.
Neurons in several brain areas respond specifically to events that are “rewarding.” A reward
makes you more likely to repeat the behavior that led to the reward; examples are food, water, sex,
and a variety of more complicated things like positive social interactions. In people, we know
rewards are associated with a subjective sense of pleasure, and people, like other animals, are
willing to work for them (as well as for human-specific rewards like money). However,
opportunities to record the responses of individual neurons in humans are rare, so studies of this sort
are typically done with rodents or monkeys.
Practical tip: How to increase your happiness
Happiness is a moving target. Because of adaptation, frequent small positive events
have a greater cumulative impact than occasional large positive events. Similarly, the
elimination of daily irritants like commuting is likely to provide a substantial improvement
in happiness. It’s hard to believe that it would make you happier to spend fifteen minutes
every evening for the rest of your life having a relaxed drink with a sympathetic friend than
it would to win the lottery, but it’s almost certainly true.
What makes people happy day to day? Women who were asked to recall their emotions
at the end of each day rated having sex as the most rewarding activity, considerably ahead
of the runner-up, socializing with friends. Indeed, more sex correlates with more happiness
—and unlike money, the happiness-producing effects of sex do not diminish once you have
enough of it. How well people slept the previous night has a stronger correlation with their
enjoyment of the day than their household income. Setting realistic goals and achieving
them is also associated with happiness for most people. You probably don’t need to worry
too much about varying your daily routine, as people who stick with their old favorites are
happier than people who seek variety for its own sake.
The study of happiness is still in its infancy, but a few researchers have shown that
behavioral exercises can increase happiness. The exercises are most effective if you do
them consistently. Here are a few of the exercises that work:
• Focus on positive events.
Every evening for a month, write down three good things
that happened that day and explain what caused each of them. This exercise increased
happiness and reduced symptoms of mild depression within a few weeks, and the effects
lasted for six months, with particularly good outcomes for people who continued to do the
exercise.
• Practice using your character strengths.
You can find out what your strengths are by
going to
http://www.authentichappiness.org
and taking the VIA Signature Strengths
questionnaire. (The Web site is run by Martin Seligman, a well-known positive
psychologist at the University of Pennsylvania. You’ll need to register for access to the site,
but the tests are free.) Once you know your top five strengths, make a point of using one of
them in a new way every day for a week. This exercise and the previous one grew out of
Seligman’s research, as described in his book,
Authentic Happiness
.
• Remember to be grateful.
Every day write down five things that you are thankful for.
People who did this exercise for several weeks had more positive feelings and fewer
negative feelings than people who did a placebo exercise. However, we do not know
whether the effects are long-lasting, as the subjects were only followed for a month.
Scientists can distinguish between neurons that respond to rewards and neurons that respond to
other aspects of a stimulus, like taste. The reward neurons are those that stop responding when the
animal no longer wants the reward, like when a rat is no longer interested in a food because it has had
enough to eat (though presumably the food still tastes the same). These neurons are found in brain
regions like the orbitofrontal cortex, striatum, and amygdala, and they often respond not only to the
existence of a rewarding stimulus, but also to some particular characteristic of the reward. For
instance, one neuron might respond to one type of food but not another, or to a small reward but not a
large reward. Although different neurons within a given brain area have different preferences, the
same set of brain areas is active when the animal receives a lot of different rewards, from food to sex
to the opportunity to spend time with its mate.
Some such neurons release the neurotransmitter dopamine. These neurons are located in the
substantia nigra and the ventral tegmental area of the midbrain, and they project their axons to a
variety of other brain regions that contain reward-responsive neurons, including those discussed
above. These neurons seem to be specifically involved in reward prediction. Dopamine neurons are
activated by unexpected rewards. For instance, experimenters taught rats that they could press a lever
and get a reward—but only after a light was on. During the early stages of training, neurons were
active when the food arrived. Later on, after the animals knew the task, the dopamine neurons began
to fire as soon as the light went on—when the animal first knew it was going to get some food—and
they were inhibited when the food failed to show up on schedule. When enough disappointments
happened repeatedly, the neurons stopped firing in response to the light, and the animals stopped
pressing the lever. In a variety of situations, then, these neurons appear to tell animals about which
features of the environment predict when they will receive a reward.
What do dopamine or reward-responsive neurons have to do with happiness? We don’t know
how to define happiness in rats (it’s hard enough to define in people), but it does look as though
dopamine helps rats—and people—to choose behaviors that lead to positive outcomes. Evidence that
signaling reward is one of dopamine’s functions in people comes from Parkinson’s disease, a
movement disorder that involves the progressive death of dopamine-making neurons serving multiple
functions. In addition to their motor problems, Parkinson’s patients have difficulty learning through
trial and error. When medication makes their dopamine levels high, Parkinson’s patients learn more
about responses that are paired with rewards. In contrast, when patients are not taking medication,
and their dopamine levels are low, they learn more easily about responses that are paired with
negative consequences. These results suggest that dopamine is involved in learning to choose
behaviors that lead to positive outcomes, which sounds like a key ingredient in happiness to us.
Success is getting what you want. Happiness is wanting what you get.
—Unknown
What’s It Like in There? Personality
It’s never pleasant to be disliked by someone you work with, especially when that someone plays
anonymous practical jokes. However, as Shelley found, it can be a little less unpleasant when that
someone is six inches long and has no bones—indeed, has no hard parts except for a beak.
Shelley spent one summer at the Marine Biological Laboratory, a research center on Cape Cod,
Massachusetts, working with cuttlefish. Cuttlefish are members of the cephalopod family, a strange
group of big-brained, big-eyed, multilimbed marine animals; their close relatives include octopuses
and squid. That summer, Shelley spent her days in a small room with a cuttlefish in a tank next to her
while she prepared behavioral tests for the animal. One day she felt something wet on her backside.
She turned around, and saw nothing—just a cuttlefish in the tank. She assumed it was just a random
splash from the aquarium pump. As it turns out, it was a pump—just not a mechanical one. She was
splashed again several times before she realized that the water was coming from the cuttlefish itself.
All cuttlefish have a siphon that they use to send water in specific directions. This particular
cuttlefish was using its siphon on Shelley, but only when her back was turned. Somehow, it’s hard to
shake the sense that Shelley was the victim of a repeated expression of dislike by her crotchety
experimental subject.
It’s clear that individual animals have distinctive personalities, and that personality is at least
partially inherited. Dog fanciers will gladly explain in great detail the quirks of different breeds.
Pomeranians are high-strung; pugs, agreeable and unaggressive. One can see the whole range of
behavior on display on any sunny day in a dog park. Personality also varies among species: we
present exhibit A, the notable absence of cat parks.
Most of our interest in animal personality stems from our encounters with companion animals,
such as dogs and cats. But ethologists (scientists who study animal behavior) examine individual
personality and temperament in many species, from dairy goats and horses to guppies and spiders.