Read Welcome to Your Brain Online
Authors: Sam Wang,Sandra Aamodt
Tags: #Neurophysiology-Popular works., #Brain-Popular works
behavior might accompany schizophrenia or bipolar disorder—but even then only rarely.
A slightly less implausible scenario occurs in the charming
Desperately Seeking Susan
(1985), in which Rosanna Arquette plays a bored housewife who loses her memory and
experiences severe confusion. Although the selective loss of identity after a head injury is
implausible, one aspect of what happens next contains a grain of truth. A personal ad and a
found article of clothing help Arquette invent a story about her lost identity. She goes on to
assume the life and obligations of an adventuress on the lam, played by Madonna. Victims
of memory loss will often fill in lost information by creating plausible memories, an act of
confabulation that creates the illusion of normal, continuous memory.
Before 1901, the trail of the idea starts to get cold. What enterprising writer first put to paper the
thought of a head blow leading to amnesia? The notion does represent an advance: it acknowledges
the brain as the seat of thought. After all, Shakespeare presented acts of magic as agents of mental
change. Think of Titania in
A Midsummer Night’s Dream
, who is induced by the prankish Puck’s
magic love drops to fall in love with Bottom, who has the head of a donkey.
Perhaps we have unfairly made fun of these depictions of memory loss. After all, psychiatric
disorders show more diverse symptoms than the strictly neurological disorders stemming from
physical injury or disease. For instance, a psychiatric patient might show selective amnesia in very
specialized ways. Also, transient memory loss is known to occur spontaneously, possibly because of
miniature strokelike events (see
Chapter 29)
. But Hollywood usually tells us that the memory loss
starts with an injury or traumatic event, and in this regard the targets of our criticism are fair game.
Cinema may be ripe for scientific criticism, but it does provide insight into how people think the
brain works.
A conceptual underpinning to many cinematic misconceptions is an idea we will call “brains are
like old televisions.” Consider a common dramatic convention: after a blow to the head induces
memory loss, memory can be restored by a second blow to the head. The existence of this myth points
to unspoken assumptions we make about how the brain works. For the second-blow hypothesis to be
true, damage to the brain would have to be reversible. Since the likeliest cause of amnesia from a
head injury would be a fluid accumulation that pushes on the brain, a therapeutic benefit from a
second injury would be pretty unlikely, to say the least.
A likely source of the second-blow idea is our everyday experience with electronic devices,
especially old ones. It’s well known that hitting an old television in just the right way can sometimes
get it to work again. These old devices usually have loose or dirty electrical contacts, suggesting that
a properly aimed blow might help reseat a connection and thus restore function. The basic problem
here is that brains do not have loose connections as such; synapses join neurons together so tightly
that no blow, short of a totally destructive injury, would ever “loosen” them.
Many moviemakers seem to think that brains are understood and organized well enough that
neurosurgery is useful as a means of repairing memory loss. It is true that neurosurgery can reduce
immediately life-threatening conditions, such as the accumulation of fluid or a tumor that compresses
the brain. These conditions would usually be accompanied by severe confusion (as in a concussion)
or loss of consciousness. Such a surgery needs to be performed immediately after the problem occurs,
presenting screenwriters with the dilemma that the dramatic value of any amnesia would have to be
compressed into the trip from the injury site to the hospital. Otherwise neurosurgery is more likely to
be an accidental cause of memory loss than a cure for it.
Did you know? Can memories be erased?
In
Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind
(2004), the main character seeks to obliterate
memories of a relationship gone wrong by going to a professional outfit that provides such
a service for a price. In the movie, the character is strapped down and goes to sleep while
technicians rummage through his head. They play back memories and pick out the ones that
need to be erased.
One idea implicit in this sequence is that neural activity somehow encodes explicit,
movielike representations of remembered experiences. Perhaps the logic is not entirely
cracked—experience does appear to be reduced and compressed as it is converted to
something that the brain can store—but the result is not a full replay of the event (see
Chapter 1
). Recollection of a visual scene does trigger brain responses that resemble in
some ways the responses that arise from viewing a scene for the first time. Another part is
less fantastic than it may sound: the idea that one can locate an offending memory, play it
back, then erase it like an unwanted computer file. Research in the past few years suggests
that recollection of a memory also reinforces the memory. There is good evidence that we
“erase” and “rewrite” our memories every time we recall them, suggesting that if it were
ever possible to erase specific content, playing it back first might be an essential
component.
In a more realistic (but totally revolting) depiction of brain injury, we have the sequel to
The
Silence of the Lambs
(1991),
Hannibal
(2001), in which gradual invasion (oh, let’s not mince words
—the cutting up and cooking of a person’s brain) causes progressive loss of function. Putting aside
the difficulty of carrying out such brain surgery without killing the patient, here at least we have a
situation in which damage to the brain leads to proportional loss of function.
In the thicket of misleading and silly depictions of the brain in popular entertainment, a few
counterexamples stand out in which the science is accurate. Scientific accuracy is not necessary for a
satisfying dramatic experience, of course, but it does seem possible to maintain accuracy, attract
critical approbation, and experience commercial success all at once. Various brain disorders are
depicted both accurately and sympathetically in the movies
Memento
,
Sé Quién Eres
,
Finding Nemo
,
and
A Beautiful Mind
.
Memento
(2000) accurately describes the problems faced by Leonard, who has severe
anterograde amnesia. Due to a head injury, Leonard cannot form lasting new memories. In addition,
he has difficulty retaining information held in immediate memory and, when distracted, loses track of
his train of thought. The effect is cleverly induced in the viewer’s mind by showing the sequence of
events in reverse order, starting with the death of a character, and ending with a scene that reveals the
meaning of all the subsequent events.
The symptoms suffered by Leonard are similar to those experienced by people with damage to the
hippocampus and related structures. The hippocampus is a horn-shaped structure that in humans is
about the size and shape of a fat man’s curled pinkie finger; we have one hippocampus on each side
of our brains. The hippocampus and the parts of the brain that link to it, such as the temporal lobe of
the cerebral cortex, are needed for the short-term storage of new facts and experiences. These
structures also seem to be important for eventual long-term storage of memories; patients with
temporal lobe or hippocampal damage, such as from a stroke, often are unable to recall events in the
weeks and months before the damage.
In
Memento
, the accident that triggers Leonard’s amnesia is depicted with remarkable fidelity,
right down to the part of his head that receives an injury, the temporal lobe of the cortex. The resulting
loss of function is also accurate, with the possible exception that unlike many patients with similar
damage, he is aware of his problem and can describe it. The most famous patient with hippocampal
and temporal lobe damage, known only as HM, is not so lucky (or perhaps he is luckier). Since he
had an experimental surgery to prevent epileptic seizures, HM lives in a perpetual now, continually
greeting people as if for the first time, even if he has spoken to them countless times before (see
The 2000 Spanish thriller
Sé Quién Eres
(I Know Who You Are) presents the case of Mario,
whose memory loss stems from Korsakoff’s syndrome, a disorder associated with advanced
alcoholism. Mario cannot recall anything that happened to him before 1977, has difficulty forming
new memories, and is often confused. Yet his psychiatrist finds herself drawn to him. In Mario’s
case, his memory defects result from damage to his thalamus and mammillary bodies, which is caused
by thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency resulting from the long-term malnutrition that often accompanies
severe alcoholism.
A final example of memory loss in the movies comes from the animated feature
Finding Nemo
(2003). The sufferer in this case is not a human being, but a fish. Dory is friendly but has severe
difficulty forming new memories. Like Leonard, she loses her train of thought when distracted. We
could complain that it is unrealistic to expect much cognitive sophistication from a fish, but
considering the egregiousness of the worst cinematic offenses, we will score this as a minor
infraction. What is realistic in this movie is the feeling of being lost that Dory experiences as she
finds her way through life, and the way that she can be annoying, even (and perhaps especially) to
those close to her.
Did you know? Schizophrenia in the movies—
A Beautiful Mind
A Beautiful Mind
(2001) dramatizes the life of the mathematician John Nash, presenting
the experience of descending into schizophrenia in great detail. The Nash character (in a
somewhat loose adaptation of the real Nash) experiences hallucinations and starts to
imagine causal links between unrelated events. His growing paranoia about the motives of
those around him and his inability to critically reject these delusions gradually alienate him
from colleagues and loved ones.
These are classic signs of schizophrenia, a disorder that is caused by changes in the
brain induced by disease, injury, or genetic predisposition. Schizophrenia typically strikes
people in their late teens and early twenties and affects more men than women. As many as
one in one hundred people experience symptoms of schizophrenia at some point in their
lives. The hallucinations experienced by the Nash character in the movie are visual; the
real-life Nash has experienced auditory hallucinations of a similar nature.
While much of the movie is scientifically accurate, one significant error is that Nash is
cured by the love of a good woman. Schizophrenia is not a romantic event; it is a physical
disorder of the brain. Some degree of recovery is possible: patients may have periods of
normal function interspersed with symptomatic periods, and symptoms disappear in as
many as one in six schizophrenics. The reasons for remission, however, are currently not
known. The error made in the movie is reminiscent of the old myth that schizophrenia is
caused by a lack of mother love, an idea that has no support, is refuted by evidence, and
makes mothers—and other loved ones—of schizophrenics feel guilty for no good reason.
This brings us to a striking recurring theme in the accurate depiction of memory loss: the
sympathetic portrayal of the sufferer. In inaccurate depictions, the victim is often regarded as a figure
of fun or even ridicule. However, the plight of accurately portrayed sufferers is almost always
rendered poignantly and, in the best cases, captures the feeling of what it is like to have a disorder.
Thinking Meat: Neurons and Synapses
In his short story “They’re Made Out of Meat,” Terry Bisson describes alien beings with electronic
brains who discover a planet, Earth, on which the most sophisticated organisms do their thinking with
living tissue. The aliens refer to brains as “thinking meat.” (Gross, we know.) The idea that your