You Are Here (25 page)

Read You Are Here Online

Authors: Colin Ellard

BOOK: You Are Here
3.39Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

In his mammoth work
London: The Biography
, Peter Ackroyd struggles to capture a detailed impression of the grand old city by
framing its history around its labyrinthine streets and alleyways. “Chapters of history resemble John Bunyan’s little wicket gates,” he says, promising to lead us “from the narrow path in search of those heights and depths of urban experience that know no history and are rarely susceptible to rational analysis.” He makes good on his promise by taking us through almost 800 pages of a history entwined around space and place.

In a chapter on Fetter Lane, an avenue heading away from busy Fleet Street, Ackroyd documents the many uses of the space extending back to its fuzzy beginnings more than a thousand years ago. Though much of today’s lane would be unrecognizable to a nineteenth-century resident, some common threads persist. The lane has always been a border. It was where the Great Fire of 1666 stopped. It was a mixed lane of brothels, taverns, and itinerant businessmen. It was filled with residents who lived on the edge, a good number of them tumbling over it when their crimes amounted to enough to warrant summary execution on gallows hastily erected mid-street. Though present-day Fetter Lane is filled with offices and sandwich shops, it retains enough of the old marks, twists, and turns that the ancient spirit of the street and its influence on our feelings may still pulse faintly, resisting the steady pendulum beat of the wrecker’s ball.
6

Ackroyd is a modern acolyte of an older tradition of psychogeographers, mostly philosophical and literary figures who felt, and tried to describe, a connection between feeling, space, and history. Like Ackroyd, psychogeographers share a sense that those hard-to-identify impressions that we glean from cities, neighborhoods, or even individual streets have much to do with the manner in which the shape and appearance of space have influenced their uses. Collectively, this amalgam of activity and geometry continues to resonate in the present time, influencing our feelings and actions as we walk a city.

The origins of the psychogeographic enterprise take us into the fascinating but bewildering territory of mid-twentieth-century French artistic and intellectual movements. One early proponent of the idea that city spaces evoke feelings as surely as mixtures of chemicals produce drug effects was Ivan Chtcheglov. In his “Formulary for a New Urbanism,” Chtcheglov wrote that cities were inhabited by ghosts created by combinations of “shifting angles” and “receding perspectives” that “allow us to glimpse original conceptions of space.” Central to Chtcheglov’s methodology was the
derive
, a kind of unstructured wandering where one was led from place to place like a robot being carried along the streets by simple rules related to the appearance of space. Though some of Chtcheglov’s pronouncements were interesting, his contribution to our understanding of how we experience space was badly impeded by a steadily worsening mental derangement. Ultimately, Chtcheglov was institutionalized in part for plotting to blow up the Eiffel Tower because the light from it shone into his bedroom and disturbed his sleep.

Guy Debord, a clearer thinker than Chtcheglov, offered the first decent definition of psychogeography as “the study of the precise laws of and specific effects of the geographic environment … on the emotions and behaviour of individuals.” Debord saw psychogeography as an essential element of a larger political initiative steeped in Marxist doctrine and intended to remake much more than the aesthetics of Parisian street corners. Debord’s psychogeographic movement drifted into obscurity, in part because, as Merlin Coverley points out in his witty summary of the movement, the principal players spent far more of their time involved in definitional infighting than in actually carrying out the
derives
that would be required to collect any data. Later in his life, Debord appears to have lost faith in the psychogeographic
derives
to reveal any underlying principles relating space and feeling, preferring to believe that each
person’s relationship with urban space was intimate, personal, and beyond the reach of the conventional tools of science.
7

IMAGES OF CITIES

Kevin Lynch, a leading twentieth-century American urban planner and a student of Frank Lloyd Wright, took a considerably more scientific approach to the general question of how cities are represented mentally. Lynch’s concerns were more closely focused on the intelligibility of a city for the purpose of finding one’s way around than they were on how cities make us feel, yet he saw close connections between the two. Lynch is best known for his groundbreaking work
The Image of the City
, the culmination of five years of survey, observation, and questioning of the residents of three major American cities, Boston, Jersey City, and Los Angeles.
8
Lynch defined the imageability of a form as that which made the form memorable. Highly imageable forms, by virtue of their “shape, color, or arrangement,” were able to generate powerful mental images of these forms. Extensive questioning of interviewees led Lynch to propose that cities contained five main elements: paths, nodes, regions, boundaries, and landmarks. Paths are linear routes that people normally use to travel from place to place. Nodes are places of gathering, the places where things happen. Regions are sections of a city (SoHo, Beacon Hill, the Annex, Chinatown) that can be mentally represented as a single chunk of space. Boundaries are linear elements that are not used as paths, such as the edges of lakes or rivers, freeways, or railroad tracks. Finally, landmarks, as we have seen before, can be local elements that symbolize the meaning, use, or feeling of a part of a city or they can be large structures (such as the Sydney Opera House or Manhattan’s Empire State Building) that are visible from many places and can act as navigational aids.

The main way in which cities varied, Lynch argued, was the extent to which each of these five elements were imageable, and he argued that the tools of urban planning could and should be used to influence this imageability. Chief among the evidence for image-ability of city elements was the frequency with which they showed up on sketch maps of cities that Lynch asked people on the street to draw for him. Though many features in each of Lynch’s three studied cities did seem to have high imageability, these sketch maps showed little sign of metric accuracy. The maps were topologically accurate, but distances were distorted, often in such a way as to suggest that the mental representations of the more imageable elements were expanded considerably.

Lynch’s work takes us away from descriptions of how city space works that are concerned only with attraction, preference, and feeling and toward a consideration of how we cope with problems of space and wayfinding in cities. Although he emphasized that the strong impressions made by highly imageable city spaces are likely to be pleasant ones and well worth pursuing on entirely aesthetic grounds, another major part of his argument was that highly image-able cities are friendlier for the wayfinder. A navigator confronted with a morass of indistinguishable streets with little visual appeal or interest not only is likely to feel unhappy to be there but is also much more likely to become lost than a navigator in well-structured, highly integrated space filled with prominent visual features.

An entirely different approach to understanding urban behavior from the intensely introspective walks of the
derive
or the analysis of sketch maps generated by residents is to adopt the simple observational methods used by many other behavioral scientists, including those whose work was described in earlier chapters on
animal behavior. If we want to understand what people do and how they feel in cities, perhaps there’s nothing better than to take up a position, as unobtrusively as possible, and to just watch them. This approach was pioneered in the United States by William Whyte.

After graduating from Princeton in 1939 and serving in the U.S. Marines during World War II, Whyte landed a good job at
Fortune
magazine and quickly rose through the ranks to an editorial position that gave him a chance to record in print his astute observations on the sociological upheavals occurring in postwar North America. From these writings eventually emerged the enormously successful book
The Organization Man
, whose recognition allowed Whyte to leave
Fortune
and devote himself full time to the study of urban behavior. In 1970, Whyte formed the Street Life Project, a small group devoted to using firsthand observational methods, including unobtrusive time-lapse photography and simple turnstile counts showing where, when, and how people spent time in cities.
9

One of Whyte’s first observations was deceptively simple. New York City contained a number of pedestrian plazas. Some of them were built at great expense, yet were always empty. Others were always teeming with business. Why? Whyte’s group tackled the question with characteristic straightforwardness. They mounted high cameras to record movements of people, and they waded into the crowds to ask questions. Why had people come? Where had they come from? Why had they chosen this particular place? Like Jane Jacobs, his finest and most successful student, Whyte learned that life attracts life. In spite of what many builders had believed, people do not look for out-of-the-way, secluded spots in cities. More than anything else, we are fascinated by each other. We want to be as close to the fray as possible. Plazas that are close to major navigational routes are much more likely to be used than those that aren’t. In a cycle of positive feedback, a small nucleus of people draws in a
further influx of bodies as if by magnetic attraction. In no time at all, one plaza is filled while another, perhaps offering all the same amenities (except for the people), remains unused.

Whyte’s observation, and many others like it, suggests that, just as we saw in the interior spaces of our houses, offices, and institutional buildings, the shape of space has a formative influence on where we go and how we gather. Route and enclosure move us from place to place with the same sureness with which a well-designed decanter pours oil onto a plate.

In his book
Architecture and the Urban Experience
, Raymond Curran uses exactly this analogy to explain how space moves us. Curran argues that all exterior spaces can be divided into receptacles, or “cluster spaces,” in which we gather and avenues, or “linear spaces,” that bring us to these gathering places. We are led along paths by our eyes, so artful presentation of contours, surface patterns, and colors can draw our attention with all the careful deliberation of a magician using sleight of hand. Spaces filled with horizontal contours sweep our eyes along, pulling our body behind them. High building facades and towers interrupt this sweeping gaze, causing us to slow and linger.
10

Curran’s illustrations make a convincing argument that in the design of cities, the spaces that lie between buildings are every bit as important as the buildings themselves. Danish architect Jan Gehl made similar arguments in his classic book
Life between Buildings
and then went on to show the city of Copenhagen how to put such principles into effect in spectacular fashion.
11
In the 1950s, like many other cities all across the world, Copenhagen found its dense core becoming choked with cars and empty of public spaces. Beginning with the placement of sidewalk cafés in the 1960s, Copenhagen’s city fathers began a protracted campaign to resurrect public space in the city. With Gehl’s help, this campaign reached its height
with the opening of a major car-free pedestrian zone, the Strøget, which attracts throngs of visitors and residents and has become the crowning jewel of the city. The tactics used by Gehl to invite people back into city cores on foot have been so effective that now other cities, most recently New York, are trying to emulate them. At some point a new verb, “to copenhagenize,” has entered the lexicon to describe the measures recommended by Gehl. In addition to an outright ban on automobiles on certain key thoroughfares, these measures include dedicated bicycle lanes, public squares, and restrictions on heights of buildings to enhance both the appearance and the climate of central pedestrian zones.

Copenhagen has become a model city for those who are trying to find ways to break down our car-centric way of life and to bring people back into the city core not just for commerce but for pleasure. Much of this evolution has been due to the steady influence of Gehl’s approach, which combines insights into human psychology and sociology with sound architectural and urban-planning principles.

UNDERSTANDING THE GRAMMAR OF CITY SPACES

The kinds of on-the-street observations made by Jacobs, Whyte, Curran, Gehl, and many others have brought about workable principles that can effect positive changes in the shapes and arrangements of urban places. If such principles could be embedded in a more thoroughgoing theoretical analysis of lived space, one that made connections to the psychology of spatial cognition, then what has worked so successfully in some of the world’s great places could perhaps be applied more systematically to our ailing cities. Bill Hillier, an architect and planner at the Bartlett School of Planning in London, has pioneered exactly such an approach to the understanding of city space.
12
Hillier has uncovered some powerful mathematical principles that can help to predict how
our movements through space are partially determined by its shape. As surely as a thin-stemmed pitcher can be used to mete out minute quantities of rare truffle oil, a large piece of real estate can be carved up in such a way as to pull us through space in any desired manner.

Other books

30 Pieces of Silver by Carolyn McCray
Forged in Fire by J.A. Pitts
Chasing Butterflies by Amir Abrams
London Falling by T. A. Foster
The Book Keeper by Amelia Grace
The Billionaire's Desire by Lambert, Kate
Extra Time by Morris Gleitzman
Paradox by John Meaney