1996 - The Island of the Colorblind (20 page)

BOOK: 1996 - The Island of the Colorblind
3.51Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

 

Then, in 1986, his eye was caught by a letter in the
Lancet
which resurrected the cycad hypothesis. Peter Spencer, a neurotoxicologist, using a purified form of the amino acid BMAA from cycad seeds, found that it could induce a neurological syndrome in monkeys, conceivably analogous to human lytico.

Spencer’s work in this realm went back to the 1970
s
, when, with his colleague Herb Schaumburg, he had travelled to India to investigate the neurolathyrism there. It had been known for centuries that a spastic paralysis of the legs could follow continued eating of the chickling pea; that this was due to the neurotoxic amino acid BOAA, which damaged the cortical motor cells and their descending connections in the spinal cord, had been known since the 1960
s
. Spencer’s new studies made clear how BOAA heightened sensitivity to glutamate, one of the neurotransmitters involved in the motor system, and simulated its action as well. BOAA intoxication, therefore, could push the glutamate receptor cells into a sort of overdrive, until they literally died of overexcitation and exhaustion. BOAA was an ex-citotoxin – this was the new term. Could BMAA, he wondered, so similar in structure to BOAA, also act as an excitotoxin and produce a disorder like lytico?

There had been attempts to induce such disorders in animal experiments during the sixties, but the results were inconclusive, and this line of research had been dropped. Now, using cynomolgus monkeys and repeated administrations of BMAA, Spencer succeeded, after eight weeks, in inducing ‘a degenerative motor system disease’ associated with damage to the motor cells in the cerebral cortex and spinal cord.
70
He further observed that BMAA might have two distinct effects: given in high doses, it caused an ALS-like condition to develop rapidly; but smaller doses seemed to cause, after a considerably longer period, a parkinsonian condition – a double action reminiscent of the Guam disease.

These results seemed to refute the first criticism made in the 1960
s
of the cycad hypothesis – that there existed no animal model. Now Spencer, with a characteristic burst of energy, set about refuting the other, seemingly lethal criticism of the cycad hypothesis – that there were no cycads in the Kii Peninsula or Irian Jaya. Like Gajdusek had before him, he trekked into the jungles of Irian Jaya with his colleague Valerie Palmer – and they discovered that there
were
cycads here (though they seemed to be a different species from the Guam one). They found, moreover, that cycads were treated as veritable medicine cabinets by the local people, who used raw seeds (like the Chamorros) as poultices on open wounds. On the Kii peninsula, they went on to discover, cycads were also used medicinally, as tonics. With these two discoveries, in the lab and the field, the cycad hypothesis, discarded fifteen years earlier, was now revivified.

John could not contain his excitement at these new thoughts and findings – everything seemed to fit together perfectly. He would phone Spencer in New York, and the two would have excited conversations for hours, sometimes nightly, discussing clinical data, and bringing out more and more ‘coincidences’ of cycads and disease in the Marianas. With his colleague Tomasa Guzman, John now embarked on reexamining the whole question of cycad distribution and use in the Marianas. They observed that while lytico-bodig was common among the Chamorros on Guam and Rota, where cycads were plentiful, there was no lytico-bodig reported on the island of Saipan (at least none in the previous seventy years – it remained uncertain whether the Saipanese Chamorros had been prone to it before this).
71
But they pointed out that the cycad forests of Saipan had been cut down by the Japanese in 1914, to clear land for sugar cane, and that the use of fadang had ceased soon after this. And that on lytico-bodig – free Tinian, where there were forests of cycads, the Chamorros had never made use of them. They proposed that the family clusters of disease found on Guam, which did not follow any known genetic distribution, could be related to differences in the way each family prepared their fadang – some family recipes involved soaking the seeds overnight, some for three weeks; some would use seawater, some fresh; some would shorten the washing process so that the flour would have a stronger taste. Steele and Guzman ended their paper with some striking accounts of people who had developed lytico-bodig as long as twenty years after a single exposure to fadang.

But many researchers felt, after the first flush of enthusiasm, that the amounts of BMAA Spencer was feeding his monkeys were completely unphysiological – more than the most devoted fadang eater could consume in a lifetime. Indeed, Gajdusek calculated, to reproduce Spencer’s experiment in a human being, the subject would have to eat a ton and a half of unprocessed cycad seeds in twelve weeks. This in itself was not an annihilating criticism – experimental toxicology often uses massive doses of materials in its initial experiments in order to increase the chance of getting results within a reasonable time. But now John, knowing how meticulously the seeds were detoxified before the production of fadang, set about measuring the amount of BMAA the flour actually contained; he started sending samples out for analysis, and was surprised to find that many of these had very low levels of BMAA, and some almost none at all. With this he turned against the cycad hypothesis, which had so exercised him for more than three years – turned against it with the vehemence with which he had once espoused it.

Gajdusek and his group, meanwhile, had also been trying to produce an animal model for lytico-bodig and had been maintaining a number of macaques on a low-calcium, high-aluminum diet. The monkeys developed no clinical symptoms in the four years of the trial, but autopsies showed many neurofibrillary tangles, as well as degenerative changes in the motor neurons, throughout the neuraxis. These changes seemed to resemble those of lytico-bodig or the presymptomatic changes described by Anderson and Chen, and it was speculated that a longer period of calcium deficiency, or higher doses of toxic metals, might have led to overt clinical disease. And though Gajdusek had told John in 1983 that he thought the lytico-bodig was dying out in Guam, he has continued to investigate it in Irian Jaya, where in 1993 he found it still had a remarkably high incidence. He and his colleagues continue to see aluminum neurotoxicity as the cause of lytico-bodig and indeed of a wide range of other conditions.

While Spencer, for his part, was greatly encouraged by his own success in inducing neurological disorders in primates with BMAA, he soon developed reservations. The disorders shown by his monkeys were dose-related, came on promptly, and were acute and nonprogressive (they resembled, in this way, the neu-rocycadism of cattle); whereas human lytico-bodig, it was abundantly clear, had a very long latency or incubation period, but once it had become symptomatic, was almost invariably progressive. Was it possible, Spencer speculated, that another factor was involved besides the BMAA, which might not predispose to overt disorder for many years? Slow viruses had been described by Gajdusek; could there not be, analogously, a slow toxin? Spencer did not have any clear idea, at this stage, of how such a toxin might work, or any way of validating the concept.

Though Gajdusek might have been expected to be sympathetic to the idea of a slow toxin, he argued passionately against it in a sternly titled paper, ‘Cycad Toxicity Not the Cause of High-Incidence ALS⁄Parkinsonism-Dementia on Guam, Kii Peninsula of Japan, or in West New Guinea,’ asserting that such a hypothesis was, first, redundant; second, without precedent; third, without support; and fourth, impossible:

No neurotoxin has been demonstrated to give rise to fatal central nervous system disease, neurological signs and symptoms of which first start to be detectable years after exposure to the neurotoxin has ceased. In fact, we have
no
example of
any
toxin producing progressive damage to
any
organ years after last exposure to the substance…Only hypersensitivity disorders, slow infections, and genetically-timed disorders have given rise to this pattern of long delay.

Spencer, undeterred, saw Gajdusek’s words as a challenge (indeed he has cited them in several of his own papers), and continued to see his task as the search for a new kind of toxin, a new kind of toxic mechanism, hitherto unrecognized in medicine. A great deal of attention was focused, in the sixties and seventies, on carcinogenesis, the appearance of cancers, in some cases, years after an initial exposure to the carcinogen, whether radioactivity, a toxin, or a virus. It had been established, in Kur-land’s original cycad conferences, what a potent carcinogen
cycasin
was, capable of inducing liver cancers and colon and kidney malformations. It had been observed, moreover, that if infant rats were fed cycasin-high diets, the still-dividing Purkinje cells of the cerebellum might develop bizarre multinucleated forms and ectopic ‘nests,’ and such findings had also been reported, on occasion, in cases of human lytico-bodig.

What then might be the effect of cycasin, Spencer wondered, on adult nerve cells, which are no longer capable of dividing?

He and Glen Kisby have postulated recently that cycasin (or its component, MAM, or methazoxymethanol) may be able to form stable compounds with the DNA in nerve cells (such adduct formation is believed to underlie the overt carcinogenic and teratogenic effects of cycasin elsewhere in the body). This aberrant DNA in the nerve cells, he thinks, could lead to subtly but persistently altered metabolic functions, the nerve cells finally becoming oversensitive to their own neurotransmitters, their own glutamate, so that this itself could act as an excitotoxin. No external agent would be needed to provoke a neurological disaster at this point, for in this pathologically sensitized state, even normal neural functioning would now overexcite neurotransmitter receptor cells and push them toward their own destruction.

The notion of such a gene toxin is not as outlandish as it seemed a decade ago, and Spencer and Kisby have now observed DNA changes in tissue cultures of cells exposed to cycasin which suggest that such a mechanism may be at work in lytico-bodig. Such a gene toxin would actually alter the genetic character of the nerve cells it affected, producing, in effect, a genetically-based form of hypersensitivity disorder.

Now that Spencer was pondering the possible effects of cycasin on adult nerve cells, he had new analyses made of traditionally prepared cycad flours and found (contrary to what John had found earlier) that the Guam samples, even though low in BMAA, contained substantial amounts of cycasin. The highest levels of cycasin, indeed, were found in samples from villages with the greatest prevalence of lytico-bodig, lending strong circumstantial support to the hypothesis of cycasin toxicity.
72

 

John is a very vivid storyteller, and as he told me this story – a story not just of a scientific odyssey, but of his own most passionate hopes and disappointments – he seemed to relive it with almost unbearable intensity. He had enjoyed a cordial relationship with Kurland and Gajdusek, he thought, and a passionate one with Spencer – but when, in 1990, he gave up on the cycad hypothesis (as four years earlier he had given up on the mineral hypothesis), a sense of intense isolation gripped him; he felt he was out in the cold, seen as an apostate by them all. In the early 1990
s
, he toyed with a viral hypothesis (this was very much in his mind when we first met, in 1993). But as a primary physician, a general practitioner, living and working amidst the whole affected community in Umatac, he has been forced to think in terms of the entire families or clans with lytico-bodig under his care – no external cause alone, it seemed clear, could adequately account for such a pattern of distribution. Had a genetic theory been thrown out prematurely? Much had changed since Kurland and Mulder first considered, then rejected, this in the 1950
s
. The classic Mendelian patterns of inheritance had now been joined by concepts of complex inheritance involving the presence of multiple genetic abnormalities and their interactions with each other and with environmental factors. Further, it was now possible to directly examine the genetic material with molecular biology, using technologies and concepts not available to the early investigators.

Working with Verena Keck, an anthropologist, John started to collect pedigrees of every patient he had seen – pedigrees of unprecedented accuracy and detail, including medical histories going back fifty years. The more pedigrees he obtained, the more he became convinced that there had to be some genetic predisposition, or perhaps several predispositions – for it looked as though the lytico and the bodig had different patterns in different families. Sometimes one saw a family in whom the affected members had only the lytico, sometimes a family in which the clinical expression was always bodig, and sometimes, more rarely, a family with both. The similarity of the pathological pictures in lytico and bodig, he started to feel, might have been misleading them all; genealogically, they seemed to be two separate diseases.

Recently John has embarked on a new series of studies, collecting DNA samples from all of his patients and sending them out for genetic analysis. He has been very excited by preliminary results indicating the presence of a genetic marker in several cases of bodig – a marker which seems to be absent in lytico and normal controls. His immediate reaction has been one of exuberance: ‘I feel the excitement coming again, and it’s a feeling I have not had since ‘86, when I was captivated by Spencer’s hypothesis.’ But it is an exuberance tempered by considerable caution (‘I don’t quite know what it means’). The search for genetic markers is extraordinarily laborious and difficult – it took more than a decade of incessant work to find a marker for Huntington’s chorea – and John is not sure whether these preliminary results will be borne out. (And even if a clear genetic basis is established for lytico and for bodig, John feels, this will indicate no more than a vulnerability or disposition; he has never doubted that some external agent is also necessary.)

Other books

A Dom for Christmas by Raven McAllan
Two Weeks in August by Nat Burns
Hunting and Gathering by Anna Gavalda
Lush in Lace by A.J. Ridges
touch my heart by jordan, wayne
The Last Dragonslayer by Jasper Fforde
War Plan Red by Peter Sasgen
Scream of Eagles by William W. Johnstone
Brave New World by Aldous Huxley