Read Delusions of Gender Online

Authors: Cordelia Fine

Delusions of Gender (25 page)

BOOK: Delusions of Gender
6.68Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

This of course does not mean that all reported activations are spurious. It just highlights the importance of being aware of the possibility. We might be more confident that Witelson’s study genuinely identified brain regions that function differently in the two sexes during emotion recognition tasks if at least some of the brain regions that showed sex differences in activation in the easy emotion-matching tasks also turned up in the harder task.
34
However, if you look back at the list of brain activations you’ll see that in neither men nor women was any brain region activated more during both the easy and difficult emotion-matching tasks.

But even if we assume that results such as these are reliable, what do they tell us about male/female differences in psychology? Does it mean that men are more analytic, if their left inferior frontal gyrus activates more, or that women are more emotional because the right amygdala is on fire? Inferring a psychological state from brain activity (like
The amygdala was activated so that means our participants were fearful
) is known as reverse inference, and as any neuroimager will tell you, it is fraught with peril.
35
Some neuroscientists have even died while making reverse inferences. Actually, I made that last bit up, but as we will see, it is extremely tricky. There are two ways that males and females can diverge in brain activation: how much activation is seen and where that activation is. Neither piece of information, unfortunately, tells us much about psychological sex differences.

Just as bigger doesn’t necessarily mean better with regards to the size of brain structures, neither does more activation necessarily mean better or psychologically more. Researchers who study development, or learning, sometimes find that some patterns of activation reduce, or become more streamlined, as development or expertise proceeds.
36
Bizarrely, activation isn’t even a surefire sign that the activity is doing
anything
useful. For example, Chris Bird and colleagues studied a patient who suffered extensive damage to the medial prefrontal cortex following a stroke. The scope of the damage included pretty much all of the brain regions that have been reliably activated in literally dozens of functional imaging
studies of mind reading. Yet the patient was fine at mind reading! As the researchers note, ‘the data reported here urge caution in concluding that medial frontal cortex is critical for effecting ToM [theory of mind]’.
37
Vision scientist Giedrius Buracas and colleagues had an equally surprising finding. They found that brain region V1 was activated more than region MT in a motion perception task. Yet it’s well-established from neurophysiological research with primates that MT – which was activated less – is critically involved in motion detection, while V1 – which was activated more – is not.
38
These two studies serve as warning flags: even though a part of the brain might light up during a task, it may not be especially or crucially involved.

The location of activation in the brain is also surprisingly uninformative. Clearly, the whole brain isn’t involved in doing everything. Different parts of the brain are specialised for processing different sorts of information. But a particular cortical region or population of neurons can be specialised for different jobs in different contexts. As imaging experts Karl Friston and Cathy Price put it, specialisation is dynamic and context-dependent.
39
For example, a particular population of neurons in the temporal cortex may, at different times, represent both identity (
Whose face is it?
) and expression (
Is it happy or sad?
). What those neurons are doing depends both on what sort of information is being fed in, and also what sort of information is being fed back from higher regions in the processing chain. ‘Specialisation is therefore not an intrinsic property of any region’, argue Price and Friston, and that means that seeing a brain region in action doesn’t mean you know what it’s up to in your particular task. For many parts of the brain, this problem is acute. For example, the anterior cingulate is activated by so many tasks that one cognitive neuroscientist I know refers to it as the ‘on button’.

There just isn’t a simple one-to-one correspondence between brain regions and mental processes, which can make interpreting imaging data a difficult task. As Jonah Lehrer recently explained in the
Boston Globe
:

[O]ne of the most common uses of brain scanners – taking a complex psychological phenomenon and pinning it to a particular bit of cortex – is now being criticized as a potentially serious oversimplification of how the brain works.… [C]ritics stress the interconnectivity of the brain, noting that virtually every thought and feeling emerges from the crosstalk of different areas spread across the cortex.
40

If so, the familiar spots of colour on brain activation maps (derided by some as ‘blobology’), labelled as male-female difference in activation, are going to tell a very oversimplified story, and one in which much of the important information may be lost. It’s also a story that, as neuropsychologist Anelis Kaiser and colleagues point out, is geared to emphasise difference over similairy.
41

Then, there is the sad fact that, at its most precise, functional imaging technology averages over a few seconds the activity of literally millions of neurons that can fire up to a hundred impulses a second. (For PET the time-scale is even longer.) ‘Using fMRI to spy on neurons is something like using Cold War–era satellites to spy on people: Only large-scale activity is visible’, says
Science
journalist Greg Miller.
42
This severely limits the interpretations that can be made about brief psychological events. Understandably, given all these interpretative gaps, many neuroscientists hesitate to speculate what their data might mean in terms of sex differences in thinking. Many, to their credit, have performed admirably as The Voice of Restraint in popular articles about gender and the brain, and in their academic work explicitly warn against making unwarranted inferences (pleas that, in certain quarters, fall on deaf ears).

It’s not, by the way, my intention to present myself as a neuroscience sceptic. Not only are some of my best friends, as well as family members, neuroimagers, but I also think that neuroscience is an extremely exciting and promising field, and can be usefully employed in combination with other techniques. I also understand that speculation is an important part of the scientific process. Nor is the topic of gender difference by any means the only area in
which overinterpretation can occur. And I certainly don’t think that research into sex differences in the brain is wrong or pointless. There
are
sex differences in the brain (although, as we’ve seen, agreeing on what these are is harder than you might think);
43
there are sex differences in vulnerabilities to certain psychological disorders, and hopefully greater understanding of the former might help to illuminate the latter. My point is simply this: that neither structural nor functional imaging can currently tell us much about differences between male and female minds. As Rutgers University psychologist Deena Skolnick Weisberg has recently argued, we should ‘remember that neuroscience, as a method for studying the mind, is still in its infancy. It shows much promise to be someday what many people want to make it into now: a powerful tool for diagnosis and research. We should remember that it has this promise, and give it the time it needs to achieve its potential – without making too much of it in the meantime.’
44

Are early twenty-first-century neuroscientific explanations of inequality – too little white matter, an unspecialised brain, too rapacious a corpus callosum – doomed to join the same garbage heap as measures of snout elongation, cephalic index and brain fibre delicacy? Will future generations look back on early twenty-first-century interpretations of imaging data with the same shocked amusement with which we regard early twentieth-century speculations about the relevance of sex differences in spinal cord size? I suspect they will, although only time will tell. But to any scientist considering trying to relate sex differences in the brain to complex psychological functions … well, let’s just say, ‘Remember Dr. Charles Dana’.

And it
is
important to remember him. For as we’ll see in the next chapter, the speculations of a few scientists quickly evolve into the colourful fabrications of popular neurosexism – the subspecialty within the larger discipline of neurononsense to which we now turn.

M
y husband would probably like you to know that, for the sake of my research for this chapter, he has had to put up with an awful lot of contemptuous snorting. For several weeks, our normally quiet hour of reading in bed before lights out became more like dinnertime in the pigsty as I worked my way through popular books about gender difference. As the result of my research, I have come up with four basic pieces of advice for anyone considering incorporating neuroscientific findings into a popular book or article about gender: (1) unless you have a time machine and have visited a future in which neuroscientists can make reverse inferences without the nagging anxieties that keep the more thoughtful of them awake at night, do not suggest that parents or teachers treat boys and girls differently because of differences observed in their brains; (2) if you don’t know what a reverse inference is, read the previous chapter of this book; (3) exercise extreme caution when making the perilous leap from brain structure to psychological function; and (4) don’t make stuff up.

When it comes to selecting examples from those who have failed to follow one or more of these four simple rules, one’s choices abound. Possibly my favourite illustration of a self-serving projection of prejudices onto brain jargon is a section in John Gray’s
Why Mars and Venus Collide
in which he discusses the inferior parietal lobe (IPL). In men, says Gray, the left IPL is more developed, while in women it is the right side that is larger. It will be no surprise to anyone, I am sure, to learn that ‘[t]he left side of the
brain has more to do with more linear, reasonable, and rational thought, while the right side of the brain is more emotional, feeling, and intuitive.’ But it
is
extraordinary just how differently the IPL serves its master and its mistress. According to Gray a man’s large left IPL, being involved in the ‘perception of time’, explains why he becomes impatient with how long a woman talks. By contrast, the IPL also ‘allows the brain to process information from the senses, particularly in selective attention, like when women are able to respond to a baby’s crying in the night.’
1
Perhaps deliberately, we are left in the dark as to whether the male inferior parietal lobe enables a man to do the same.

In
Leadership and the Sexes
, Michael Gurian and Barbara Annis inform executives that ‘women’s brains tend to link more of the emotional activity that is going on in the middle of the brain (the limbic system) with thoughts and words in the top of the brain (the cerebral cortex). Thus a man might need many hours to process a major emotion-laden experience [
I

just

got

fired
.…
I

am

sad

and

angry
.], whereas a woman may be able to process it quite quickly [
Oh, crap!
].’
2
A further neurophysiological disadvantage for men may be found in another of Gurian’s books,
What Could He Be Thinking?
Implicitly drawing on a working metaphor of
The Brain as Pinball Machine
, he explains how in men the ‘signal’ of an emotional feeling, having made it to the right hemisphere, ‘may well get stopped, disappearing into neural oblivion because the signal found no access to a receptor in a language center in the left side of the brain.’ This doesn’t happen in the female brain because, according to Gurian, while men have just one or two language centres in the left hemisphere, women have as many as seven such centres, dotted all over the brain, as well as a 25 percent larger corpus callosum. (Despite this embarrassment of neurological riches, the contrast Gurian draws between male and female brain function leaves me speechless.) And so, in men, a feeling signal is much less likely to hit the jackpot of contact with a neuron involved in language.
3

We also discover in
Leadership and the Sexes
that when a woman
leader asks her colleagues, ‘What do you all think?’ this is a typically female ‘white matter’ question. It seems that white matter isn’t just involved in integrating information from different parts of the brain, but also from different people in the office.
4
Brain differences may also be behind a female-leadership problem-solving style: when a female leader ‘knows what to do, she’s not as worried as a man might be about proving it with data’. Gurian and Annis suggest that ‘[o]ne reason for this intuitiveness may be that she has a larger
corpus callosum
connecting both hemispheres of the brain’. By contrast, male leaders favour a problem-solving style that, in part, ‘relies on more linear data and proof.’
5

BOOK: Delusions of Gender
6.68Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Big Sky Wedding by Linda Lael Miller
Doomed Queen Anne by Carolyn Meyer
Dodger of the Dials by James Benmore
Greta Again! by Stones, Marya