Read Molecular Gastronomy: Exploring the Science of Flavor Online
Authors: Hervé This
Tags: #Cooking, #General, #Methods, #Essays & Narratives, #Special Appliances, #Science, #Chemistry, #Physics, #Technology & Engineering, #Food Science, #Columbia University Press, #ISBN-13: 9780231133128
the intracellular electrical potential reaches a certain threshold. Nonetheless,
an upper limit appears to exist as well: Frogs’ egg cells equipped with the vr1
receptor die after a few hours of continuous exposure to capsaicin, evidently
because the inflow of calcium ions is excessive.
The loss of sensitivity observed in spice lovers seems to result from the
death of sensory fibers. This would explain the paradoxically analgesic effect
of capsaicin in the treatment of viral and diabetic neuropathies and of rheu-
matoid arthritis, where by killing pain neurons it helps reduce the sensation
of pain.
Finally, the ucsf team showed that rapid increases in temperature trigger
ion currents in the vr1 receptor analogous to the currents triggered by capsa-
icin. The vr1 channel therefore turns out to be both a chemical and a thermal
sensor, which is why eating spicy dishes makes the mouth feel as though it is
on fire.
Hot Up Front
| 105
28
The Taste of Cold
Cooling and heating the tongue arouse the perception of tastes, even in the
absence of food.
t h e p h y s i o l og y o f f l a vor is riding a wave of fresh discoveries. In
recent years physiologists have elucidated the molecular bases of the sensa-
tion of spiciness, described the mechanisms in the papillary cells of the
mouth responsible for taste perception, and at long last, in April 2000,
identified the first receptor for a taste molecule. It was expected that the
analytical methods that led to this last discovery would help researchers find
other receptors, but a great surprise lay in store: thermal tastes. Variations
in the temperature of the tongue alone are enough to cause tastes to be
perceived.
When we eat, various molecules stimulate the olfactory cells of the nose,
the papillary cells of the mouth, and receptors that register levels of spici-
ness and a set of mechanical and thermal sensors. How do these different
perceptions interact to form the synthetic experience of flavor? It was long
supposed that the pieces of information obtained by the various sensory cells
traveled upward up from neuron to neuron until they reached the higher
centers of the brain, which then gave them a joint interpretation, thus creat-
ing the sensation of flavor. A few years ago, however, physiologists came to
realize that this account was inadequate: Sensory information has already
been combined by the time it reaches the first neuronal relay station, which
is to say that sensory integration begins in the tongue.
106 |
Heating the Tongue
To determine the physiological consequences of this integration, Ernesto
Cruz and Barry Green at Yale University investigated the effect of the tempera-
ture of foods on taste perception. Using small thermodes (devices whose tem-
perature is regulated by means of electric currents) to stimulate the tongues
of subjects, they discovered—rediscovered, actually—the “thermal tastes”
aroused by heating and cooling the tongue. This effect had been discovered in
1964 by G. von Békésy, but the observation was part of a theory that was later
discredited and so failed to receive the attention it deserved.
The reactions were not identical for all subjects, but for a large proportion
of them, heating the tip of the tongue up to 35°c (95°f) produced a slight sweet
sensation, and cooling it down to 5°c (41°f) elicited a sour sensation or, in the
case of one person, a salt taste. Heating the back of the tongue produced only
a weak sensation of sweetness, but cooling this region often gave rise to more
distinct sensations, variously described as bitter or sour. A few subjects tested
perceived none of these thermal flavors.
Plainly this curious phenomenon called for further scrutiny. The Yale physi-
ologists sought first to quantify the relationship between temperature and the
intensity of sensations: The intensity of the sweet flavor, at the front of the
tongue, increases as the temperature rises, and the sour taste caused by cool-
ing becomes saltier as the temperature falls. Because the subjects often re-
ported differences in taste perception between the center and the sides of the
tongue, Cruz and Green sought to make these impressions more precise. They
found that for all subjects the perception of thermal sweetness was greatest in
the tip of the tongue and that thermal sourness was most clearly perceived on
the sides of the tongue.
Paradox Elucidated
How are these results to be interpreted? We know that the nerve fibers of
two cranial nerves innervate the gustatory papillae. The fibers of the chorda
tympani are ramified in the papillae and transmit information about tastes to
the brain, whereas the fibers of the lingual branch of the trigeminal nerve termi-
nate in the papillary epithelium, near the taste buds, and transmit information
The Taste of Cold
| 107
about temperature, pain, irritation, and pressure. Note that thermal sensors and
taste receptors are found near one another in the mouth.
It seems plausible to suppose that changes in temperature activate the re-
ceptors responsible for the normal coding of the perception of tastes. In this
hypothesis, it ought to be possible to block thermal tastes by stimulating taste
receptors. Conversely, if the perception of thermal flavors is not caused by the
normal taste receptors, inactivating these receptors will have no effect on the
perception of thermal taste. Tests are under way.
What are cooks to make of this discovery? One does not taste with the tip
of the tongue alone, so the effect of thermal tastes is weak in ordinary eating
situations. On the other hand, it is a simple matter to determine whether you
are capable of perceiving such flavors: Just place an ice cube against the tip of
your tongue or stick your tongue in a glass of warm water.
108 | t he physiology of f l a vor
29
Mastication
Understanding how we chew our food will change how we think about
cooking.
d r . j o h n h a r v e y k e l l o g g, he of the breakfast cereals, advocated a hy-
gienic regime based on relentless mastication. His ideas echoed an ancient
East Asian tradition according to which each mouthful of whole-grain rice was
to be chewed 100 times.
Why do we chew in the first place? Everyone knows that mastication breaks
up food into smaller pieces—small enough that, having also been lubricated
by saliva, they easily descend into the digestive system. Jons Prinz and Peter
Lucas at the Odontological Museum in London have identified another func-
tion. Without knowing it, we chew until particles of food are bound together
by saliva into a compact mouthful that can be swallowed in such a way as to
minimize the risk that small bits take a wrong turn down into the windpipe.
For each food, then, there is an optimal number of masticatory movements.
In asserting that “animals feed, man eats,” Brillat-Savarin sought to do away
with the animal side of our nature—the very thing that upset the
Précieuses
of
mid–seventeenth-century salons in Paris, who made a fashion of mousses be-
cause they eliminated the need for “the unsightly act of mastication.” And yet
who wants to forgo the pleasures of a piece of crusty bread? A sticky dumpling?
A crispy piece of bacon? If we are to enjoy the full range of pleasures that the
culinary world offers, we must frankly accept our humanity and turn our physi-
ological peculiarities to the advantage of our weakness for good food.
| 109
Chewing divides food into pieces of smaller diameter than that of our phar-
ynx. Nonetheless, we normally go well beyond what is necessary for this pur-
pose. As mammals that expend a great deal of energy, we chew our food in
order to increase the surface area accessible to digestive enzymes. Indirectly,
then, mastication accelerates the assimilation of nourishment.
Prinz and Lucas devised a model to explain how salivation causes the par-
ticles formed by chewing to cohere. Their model takes into account the two
main forces exerted on masticated food: adhesion between its parts and the
adhesion of these parts to the inside of the mouth. These forces depend on the
secretion of saliva and the quantity of juice squeezed out of food by the act of
chewing.
Small pieces of food are broken up less thoroughly than big pieces. On the
other hand, the number of fragments into which a mouthful of food is divided
by chewing depends on the mechanical characteristics of the food in question.
To simplify the modeling problem, the British physiologists assumed that each
piece of food is divided into spherical particles and calculated the total surface
forces holding them together.
Furthermore, Prinz and Lucas assumed that these particles agglomerate
when the force causing them to adhere to one another is greater than the force
causing them to adhere to the wall of the mouth. Using computer calcula-
tions of these forces and incorporating values for various other parameters
drawn from studies of human physiology, they then determined the cohesion
of mouthfuls of food after 150 masticatory cycles for two foods having very
different properties: raw carrots, which are broken up very slowly, and Bra-
zil nuts, which are broken up much more rapidly. Computation showed that
the cohesion of the masticated food is initially low, then rapidly increases and
reaches its highest point after twenty cycles. After that point it diminishes as
the particles become smaller and smaller.
To test the proposed model, the calculated degree of cohesion was com-
pared with the cohesion actually measured in mouthfuls of food spit out after
having been chewed. The agreement of theory with practice was good, but the
actual number of masticatory cycles was a bit higher than the number calcu-
lated, no doubt because we are not only machines for absorbing nourishment:
“The Creator, in making man eat in order to live,” Brillat-Savarin observed,
“persuaded him by appetite and rewarded with by pleasure.” Because we take
110 | t he physiology of f l a vor
pleasure in eating, we prolong our enjoyment by chewing longer than is strictly
necessary in order to make food particles cohere.
Model and Cuisine
What can we learn from the model for culinary purposes? Depending on
their physical characteristics, foods need a greater or lesser degree of mastica-
tion. The addition of compounds that make saliva more liquid (tannins, for
example) or increase the concentration of liquids extracted by the teeth has the
effect of reducing cohesion, which ought to lengthen the amount of time spent
chewing and so add to the enjoyment one takes from a dish. Could this be why
gourmets drink wine (which contains tannins) with their meals?
Thickening agents, on the other hand, ought to accelerate the absorption of
food into the digestive system. The use of such agents, particularly in diet prod-
ucts, creates a marketing problem: The shorter the time that food is chewed,
the fewer the number of odorant and taste molecules that are released.
More generally, the hypothesis that the body automatically detects the ideal
cohesion of mouthfuls of food ought to be a source of fresh ideas for the
cook who wants to find new ways to combine sticky, gluey, dry, or absorbent
ingredients.
Mastication
| 111
30
Tenderness and Juiciness
Chewing is what allows us to enjoy the juiciness and tenderness of meat—
though for different periods of time in each case.
l e s s t h a n a d e c a d e a g o tenderness was thought to be the most im-
portant sign of a good piece of meat. After all, the true gourmet detests tough
meat. But how does one tell tenderness and toughness apart? It had been
forgotten that meat is not butter and that texture is one of its fundamental
qualities. Toughness was confused with a lack of juiciness and the need to
chew for a while before swallowing. To elucidate the relationship between the
physical structure of meats and their texture, Institut National de la Recher-
che Agronomique biologists in Clermont-Ferrand analyzed the mastication of
samples of meat prepared in various ways.
Studies of the texture of meat have long been hobbled by the mistaken idea
that the texture of a food is the same thing as its consistency, which is a micro-
structural property. Texture has to do instead with the psychological reaction
to the physicochemical stimuli aroused by mastication. (For example, water is
a liquid, but if you land on it outstretched from a height it can feel as hard as
concrete: the texture of water varies depending on whether it can be displaced
beneath a falling body, but its consistency is always the same.) Sensory per-