Your Brain and Business: The Neuroscience of Great Leaders (29 page)

BOOK: Your Brain and Business: The Neuroscience of Great Leaders
8.3Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

 

 

Conclusion

 

One of the most challenging tasks for many companies is “getting to action.” If we understand how the brain operates in order to act, we would be in a much better position to institute the relevant steps to increase productivity and prevent procrastinating with ideas that never come to fruition.

 

References

 

1. Haggard, P., S. Clark, and J. Kalogeras, “Voluntary action and conscious awareness.”
Nat Neurosci
, 2002. 5(4): p. 382–5.
2. Siriopoulos, C. and P. Tziogkidis, “How do Greek banking institutions react after significant events?—A DEA approach.”
Omega
(Westport), 2010. 38(5): p. 294.
3. Lou, P., S. Ong, and A. Nee, “Agent-based distributed scheduling for virtual job shops.”
International Journal of Production Research
, 2010. 48(13): p. 3889.
4. Wallis, S., “Appreciating the Unpredictable: A Case Study on Questions.”
Organization Development Journal
, 2010. 28(2): p. 73–78.
5. Butler, M.J. and C. Senior, “Toward an organizational cognitive neuroscience.”
Ann N Y Acad Sci
, 2007. 1118: p. 1–17.
6. Butler, M.J. and C. Senior, “Research possibilities for organizational cognitive neuroscience.”
Ann N Y Acad Sci
, 2007. 1118: p. 206–10.
7. Bower, A.J., “Plasticity in the adult and neonatal central nervous system.”
Br J Neurosurg
, 1990. 4(4): p. 253–64.
8. Kolb, B. and I.Q. Whishaw, “Brain plasticity and behavior.”
Annu Rev Psychol
, 1998. 49: p. 43–64.
9. Luria, A.R.,
Higher Cortical Functions in Man, Second Edition
(B. Haigh, Trans.), 1966, New York: Basic Books, Inc.
10. Milner, B., “Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting.”
Arch. Neurol
., 1963: p. 100–110.
11. Sandson, J. and M.L. Albert, “Varieties of perseveration.”
Neuropsychologia
, 1984. 22(6): p. 715–32.
12. Duncan, J., et al., “Intelligence and the frontal lobe: the organization of goal-directed behavior.”
Cognit Psychol
, 1996. 30(3): p. 257–303.
13. Carota, F., et al., “Neural Dynamics of the Intention to Speak.”
Cereb Cortex
, 2009.
14. Andersen, R.A. and H. Cui, “Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits.”
Neuron
, 2009. 63(5): p. 568–83.
15. Astle, D.E., G.M. Jackson, and R. Swainson, “Fractionating the Cognitive Control Required to Bring About a Change in Task: A Dense-sensor Event-related Potential Study.”
J Cogn Neurosci
, 2008. 20(2): p. 255–267.
16. Uretzky, S. and A. Gilboa, “Knowing Your Lines but Missing Your Cue: Rostral Prefrontal Lesions Impair Prospective Memory Cue Detection, but Not Action-intention Superiority.”
J Cogn Neurosci
. 2010 (e-pub: ahead of publication date)
17. Ortigue, S., et al., “Spatio-temporal dynamics of human intention understanding in temporo-parietal cortex: a combined EEG/fMRI repetition suppression paradigm.”
PLoS One
, 2009. 4(9): p. e6962.
18. Gilbert, S.J., et al., “Separable brain systems supporting cued versus self-initiated realization of delayed intentions.”
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn
, 2009. 35(4): p. 905–15.
19. Rogers, R.D. and S. Monsell, “The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks.”
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen
., 1995. 124(2): p. 207–231.
20. Mayr, U. and S.W. Keele, “Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition.”
J Exp Psychol Gen
, 2000. 129(1): p. 4–26.
21. Astle, D.E., G.M. Jackson, and R. Swainson, “Dissociating neural indices of dynamic cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: an ERP study of task switching.”
Brain Res
, 2006. 1125(1): p. 94–103.
22. Staines, W.R., M. Padilla, and R.T. Knight, “Frontal-parietal event-related potential changes associated with practising a novel visuomotor task.”
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res
, 2002. 13(2): p. 195–202.
23. Mitchell, D.J. and R. Cusack, “Flexible, Capacity-Limited Activity of Posterior Parietal Cortex in Perceptual as well as Visual Short-Term Memory Tasks.”
Cereb Cortex
, 2007.
24. Landau, S.M., et al., “Regional specificity and practice: dynamic changes in object and spatial working memory.”
Brain Res
, 2007. 1180: p. 78–89.
25. Sui, J. and S. Han, “Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness.”
Psychol Sci
, 2007. 18(10): p. 861–6.
26. Winocur, G., M. Moscovitch, and M. Sekeres, “Memory consolidation or transformation: context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory.”
Nat Neurosci
, 2007. 10(5): p. 555–7.
27. Wegner, D.M., “How to think, say, or do precisely the worst thing for any occasion.”
Science
, 2009. 325(5936): p. 48–50.
28. Anderson, M.C., et al., “Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories.”
Science
, 2004. 303(5655): p. 232–5.
29. Schwabe, L. and O.T. Wolf, “Stress prompts habit behavior in humans.”
J Neurosci
, 2009. 29(22): p. 7191–8.
30. Welchman, A.E., et al., “The quick and the dead: when reaction beats intention.”
Proc Biol Sci
. 277(1688): p. 1667–74.
31. Backhaus, J. and K. Junghanns, “Daytime naps improve procedural motor memory.”
Sleep Med
, 2006. 7(6): p. 508–12.
32. Lahl, O., et al., “An ultra short episode of sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance.”
J Sleep Res
, 2008. 17(1): p. 3–10.
33. Nishida, M. and M.P. Walker, “Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles.”
PLoS One
, 2007. 2(4): p. e341.
34. Nielson, K.A. and M. Powless, “Positive and negative sources of emotional arousal enhance long-term word-list retention when induced as long as 30 min after learning.”
Neurobiol Learn Mem
, 2007. 88(1): p. 40–7.
35. Sharot, T., M. Verfaellie, and A.P. Yonelinas, “How emotion strengthens the recollective experience: a time-dependent hippocampal process.”
PLoS One
, 2007. 2(10): p. e1068.
36. Smeets, T., et al., “Context-dependent enhancement of declarative memory performance following acute psychosocial stress.”
Biol Psychol
, 2007. 76(1–2): p. 116–23.
37. Callan, D.E. and N. Schweighofer, “Positive and negative modulation of word learning by reward anticipation.”
Hum Brain Mapp
, 2008. 29(2): p. 237–49.
38. Harmon-Jones, E. and J. Mills,
Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology
. 1999, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
39. Wicklund, R.A. and J.W. Brehm,
Perspectives on Cognitive Dissonance
. 1976, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
40. Aronson, E.,
The Theory of Cognitive Dissonance: A Current Perspective. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Ed
. (L. Berkowitz.) Vol. 4., 1969, New York: Academic Press. p. 1–34.
41. Croyle, R.T. and J. Cooper, “Dissonance arousal: physiological evidence.”
J Pers Soc Psychol
, 1983. 45(4): p. 782–91.
42. Elkin, R.A. and M.R. Leippe, “Physiological arousal, dissonance, and attitude change: evidence for a dissonance-arousal link and a ‘don’t remind me’ effect.”
J Pers Soc Psychol
, 1986. 51(1): p. 55–65.
43. Carter, C.S., et al., “Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance.”
Science
, 1998. 280(5364): p. 747–9.
44. Amodio, D.M., et al., “Neural signals for the detection of unintentional race bias.”
Psychol Sci
, 2004. 15(2): p. 88–93.
45. Harmon-Jones, E., et al., “Left frontal cortical activation and spreading of alternatives: tests of the action-based model of dissonance.”
J Pers Soc Psychol
, 2008. 94(1): p. 1–15.
46. Oliveri, M., et al., “Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues.”
Exp Brain Res
, 2003. 149(2): p. 214–21.
47. Hamel, G., “Moon shots for management.”
Harv Bus Rev
, 2009. 87(2): p. 91–8.

Chapter 7. Coaching Brain Regions

 

In the book so far, we have introduced some of the critical concepts that you, as a coach or organizational developer, can use to communicate with leaders. In my experience teaching and doing this work, leaders and followers are very receptive to interventions placed within the neural framework because this framework is concrete and can be grasped more than some “emotional” and “organizational psychology” frameworks. Effective coaching involves integrating all of these approaches and having as many tools available as necessary to reach the leader.

Although we have implicated many brain regions and concepts in the various stages and aspects of coaching managers, leaders, and followers, one question remains: Can we use this information as part of an organized approach or in a systematic way (and how specifically)? The answer to this question is complex. The principles are as follows: (1) By understanding the concepts relevant to a specific phenomenon, you can prime your own brain to think of the relevant interventions from a neural perspective. (2) Because the brain regions fundamentally do the same things (the ACC is involved as a conflict detector and part of the attentional mechanism, whether we are talking about change or decision making; and the vmPFC is the accountant that weighs factual and emotional risks and benefits, whether we are talking about positive psychology or imagery), the actions with
which we target these regions are similar in nature but relate specifically to the concept relevant to your reason for being hired. Hence, I did not include this in each chapter because this would have been redundant. (3) Because each brain region may serve multiple functions, we can use a concept that I call “alternative brain probing” (ABP) or “backroading” to reach the relevant coaching targets through another pathway. For example, if an employee, manager, or leader complains that she is unable to attend to her work, you may use interventions under “fronto-parietal interventions” (increase attention), “ACC interventions” (decrease conflict, which then decreases disruption to attention), or “amygdala interventions” (decrease emotional disruptions to attention). Ordinarily, when we ignore these connections, we focus on the primary problem (in this case, attention), but when we understand these connections, we can utilize this understanding to find alternative ways (hence ABP) or backroads to stimulate this same brain region.

Thus, this chapter is divided into two sections: (1) a review of brain regions and their basic functions; (2) interventions targeted to these brain regions. One of the critiques that one must consider is that the brain does not function as independent brain regions, making an appearance when we want them to. It is most accurately understood as a network of brain regions. As such, these “regional brain interventions” are more like symptomatic “cures” in that they do not get at the causes of the behaviors directly, but in engaging these regions, we can increase the comfort as well as the intellectual and emotional functioning of the manager, worker, or leader.

The data that is missing is what coaching specifically does to the brain, and when we obtain this data, it will add to the tremendous body of knowledge we already have.

 

Review of Brain Regions and Their Basic Functions

 

 

1. The Thinking Brain

 

The success of leaders is based on their actions. Actions arise from decisions, and decisions arise from an assessment of risks and benefits, as shown in
Figure 7.1
.

 

Figure 7.1. The process of decision-making

 

 

The “thinking” part of the brain is the cerebral cortex. This is the outer part of the brain and is sometimes referred to as “neocortex.” The outer part of the brain is divided into several lobes. They are the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices (see
Figure 7.2
).

BOOK: Your Brain and Business: The Neuroscience of Great Leaders
8.3Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

ArtofDesire by Helena Harker
Realm of Light by Deborah Chester
Act of Passion by Georges Simenon
The Pink Ghetto by Ireland, Liz
Love in Lowercase by Francesc Miralles
Resistance by K Larsen