The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology (30 page)

Read The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology Online

Authors: Ray Kurzweil

Tags: #Non-Fiction, #Fringe Science, #Retail, #Technology, #Amazon.com

BOOK: The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology
12.13Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Until very recently neuroscience was characterized by overly simplistic models limited by the crudeness of our sensing and scanning tools. This led
many observers to doubt whether our thinking processes were inherently capable of understanding themselves. Peter D. Kramer writes, “If the mind were simple enough for us to understand, we would be too simple to understand it.”
50
Earlier, I quoted Douglas Hofstadter’s comparison of our brain to that of a giraffe, the structure of which is not that different from a human brain but which clearly does not have the capability of understanding its own methods. However, recent success in developing highly detailed models at various levels—from neural components such as synapses to large neural regions such as the cerebellum—demonstrate that building precise mathematical models of our brains and then simulating these models with computation is a challenging but viable task once the data capabilities become available. Although models have a long history in neuroscience, it is only recently that they have become sufficiently comprehensive and detailed to allow simulations based on them to perform like actual brain experiments.

Subneural Models: Synapses and Spines

 

In an address to the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association in 2002, psychologist and neuroscientist Joseph LeDoux of New York University said,

If who we are is shaped by what we remember, and if memory is a function of the brain, then synapses—the interfaces through which neurons communicate with each other and the physical structures in which memories are encoded—are the fundamental units of the self. . . . Synapses are pretty low on the totem pole of how the brain is organized, but I think they’re pretty important. . . .The self is the sum of the brain’s individual subsystems, each with its own form of “memory,” together with the complex interactions among the subsystems. Without synaptic plasticity—the ability of synapses to alter the ease with which they transmit signals from one neuron to another—the changes in those systems that are required for learning would be impossible.
51

Although early modeling treated the neuron as the primary unit of transforming information, the tide has turned toward emphasizing its subcellular components. Computational neuroscientist Anthony J. Bell, for example, argues:

Molecular and biophysical processes control the sensitivity of neurons to incoming spikes (both synaptic efficiency and post-synaptic responsivity), the excitability of the neuron to produce spikes, the patterns of
spikes it can produce and the likelihood of new synapses forming (dynamic rewiring), to list only four of the most obvious interferences from the subneural level. Furthermore, transneural volume effects such as local electric fields and the transmembrane diffusion of nitric oxide have been seen to influence, responsively, coherent neural firing, and the delivery of energy (blood flow) to cells, the latter of which directly correlates with neural activity. The list could go on. I believe that anyone who seriously studies neuromodulators, ion channels, or synaptic mechanism and is honest, would have to reject the neuron level as a separate computing level, even while finding it to be a useful descriptive level.
52

Indeed, an actual brain synapse is far more complex than is described in the classic McCulloch-Pitts neural-net model. The synaptic response is influenced by a range of factors, including the action of multiple channels controlled by a variety of ionic potentials (voltages) and multiple neurotransmitters and neuromodulators. Considerable progress has been made in the past twenty years, however, in developing the mathematical formulas underlying the behavior of neurons, dendrites, synapses, and the representation of information in the spike trains (pulses by neurons that have been activated). Peter Dayan and Larry Abbott have recently written a summary of the existing nonlinear differential equations that describe a wide range of knowledge derived from thousands of experimental studies.
53
Well-substantiated models exist for the biophysics of neuron bodies, synapses, and the action of feedforward networks of neurons, such as those found in the retina and optic nerves, and many other classes of neurons.

Attention to how the synapse works has its roots in Hebb’s pioneering work. Hebb addressed the question, How does short-term (also called working) memory function? The brain region associated with short-term memory is the prefrontal cortex, although we now realize that different forms of short-term information retention have been identified in most other neural circuits that have been closely studied.

Most of Hebb’s work focused on changes in the state of synapses to strengthen or inhibit received signals and on the more controversial reverberatory circuit in which neurons fire in a continuous loop.
54
Another theory proposed by Hebb is a change in state of a neuron itself—that is, a memory function in the cell soma (body). The experimental evidence supports the possibility of all of these models. Classical Hebbian synaptic memory and reverberatory memory require a time delay before the recorded information can be
used. In vivo experiments show that in at least some regions of the brain there is a neural response that is too fast to be accounted for by such standard learning models, and therefore could only be accomplished by learning-induced changes in the soma.
55

Another possibility not directly anticipated by Hebb is real-time changes in the neuron connections themselves. Recent scanning results show rapid growth of dendrite spikes and new synapses, so this must be considered an important mechanism. Experiments have also demonstrated a rich array of learning behaviors on the synaptic level that go beyond simple Hebbian models. Synapses can change their state rapidly, but they then begin to decay slowly with continued stimulation, or in some a lack of stimulation, or many other variations.
56

Although contemporary models are far more complex than the simple synapse models devised by Hebb, his intuitions have largely proved correct. In addition to Hebbian synaptic plasticity, current models include global processes that provide a regulatory function. For example, synaptic scaling keeps synaptic potentials from becoming zero (and thus being unable to be increased through multiplicative approaches) or becoming excessively high and thereby dominating a network. In vitro experiments have found synaptic scaling in cultured networks of neocortical, hippocampal, and spinal-cord neurons.
57
Other mechanisms are sensitive to overall spike timing and the distribution of potential across many synapses. Simulations have demonstrated the ability of these recently discovered mechanisms to improve learning and network stability.

The most exciting new development in our understanding of the synapse is that the topology of the synapses and the connections they form are continually changing. Our first glimpse into the rapid changes in synaptic connections was revealed by an innovative scanning system that requires a genetically modified animal whose neurons have been engineered to emit a fluorescent green light. The system can image living neural tissue and has a sufficiently high resolution to capture not only the dendrites (interneuronal connections) but the spines: tiny projections that sprout from the dendrites and initiate potential synapses.

Neurobiologist Karel Svoboda and his colleagues at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory on Long Island used the scanning system on mice to investigate networks of neurons that analyze information from the whiskers, a study that provided a fascinating look at neural learning. The dendrites continually grew new spines. Most of these lasted only a day or two, but on occasion a spine would remain stable. “We believe that the high turnover that we see might play an important role in neural plasticity, in that the sprouting spines reach out to
probe different presynaptic partners on neighboring neurons,” said Svoboda. “If a given connection is favorable, that is, reflecting a desirable kind of brain rewiring, then these synapses are stabilized and become more permanent. But most of these synapses are not going in the right direction, and they are retracted.”
58

Another consistent phenomenon that has been observed is that neural responses decrease over time, if a particular stimulus is repeated. This adaptation gives greatest priority to new patterns of stimuli. Similar work by neurobiologist Wen-Biao Gan at New York University’s School of Medicine on neuronal spines in the visual cortex of adult mice shows that this spine mechanism can hold long-term memories: “Say a 10-year-old kid uses 1,000 connections to store a piece of information. When he is 80, one-quarter of the connections will still be there, no matter how things change. That’s why you can still remember your childhood experiences.” Gan also explains, “Our idea was that you actually don’t need to make many new synapses and get rid of old ones when you learn, memorize. You just need to modify the strength of the preexisting synapses for short-term learning and memory. However, it’s likely that [a] few synapses are made or eliminated to achieve long-term memory.”
59

The reason memories can remain intact even if three quarters of the connections have disappeared is that the coding method used appears to have properties similar to those of a hologram. In a hologram, information is stored in a diffuse pattern throughout an extensive region. If you destroy three quarters of the hologram, the entire image remains intact, although with only one quarter of the resolution. Research by Pentti Kanerva, a neuroscientist at Redwood Neuroscience Institute, supports the idea that memories are dynamically distributed throughout a region of neurons. This explains why older memories persist but nonetheless appear to “fade,” because their resolution has diminished.

Neuron Models

 

Researchers are also discovering that specific neurons perform special recognition tasks. An experiment with chickens identified brain-stem neurons that detect particular delays as sounds arrive at the two ears.
60
Different neurons respond to different amounts of delay. Although there are many complex irregularities in how these neurons (and the networks they rely on) work, what they are actually accomplishing is easy to describe and would be simple to replicate. According to University of California at San Diego neuroscientist Scott Makeig, “Recent neurobiological results suggest an important role of precisely synchronized neural inputs in learning and memory.”
61

Electronic Neurons
. A recent experiment at the University of California at San Diego’s Institute for Nonlinear Science demonstrates the potential for electronic neurons to precisely emulate biological ones. Neurons (biological or otherwise) are a prime example of what is often called chaotic computing. Each neuron acts in an essentially unpredictable fashion. When an entire network of neurons receives input (from the outside world or from other networks of neurons), the signaling among them appears at first to be frenzied and random. Over time, typically a fraction of a second or so, the chaotic interplay of the neurons dies down and a stable pattern of firing emerges. This pattern represents the “decision” of the neural network. If the neural network is performing a pattern-recognition task (and such tasks constitute the bulk of the activity in the human brain), the emergent pattern represents the appropriate recognition.

So the question addressed by the San Diego researchers was: could electronic neurons engage in this chaotic dance alongside biological ones? They connected artificial neurons with real neurons from spiny lobsters in a single network, and their hybrid biological-nonbiological network performed in the same way (that is, chaotic interplay followed by a stable emergent pattern) and with the same type of results as an all-biological net of neurons. Essentially, the biological neurons accepted their electronic peers. This indicates that the chaotic mathematical model of these neurons was reasonably accurate.

Brain Plasticity

 

In 1861 French neurosurgeon Paul Broca correlated injured or surgically affected regions of the brain with certain lost skills, such as fine motor skills or language ability. For more than a century scientists believed these regions were hardwired for specific tasks. Although certain brain areas do tend to be used for particular types of skills, we now understand that such assignments can be changed in response to brain injury such as a stroke. In a classic 1965 study, D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel showed that extensive and far-reaching reorganization of the brain could take place after damage to the nervous system, such as from a stroke.
62

Moreover, the detailed arrangement of connections and synapses in a given region is a direct product of how extensively that region is used. As brain scanning has attained sufficiently high resolution to detect dendritic spine growth and the formation of new synapses, we can see our brain grow and adapt to literally follow our thoughts. This gives new shades of meaning to Descartes’ dictum “I think therefore I am.”

Other books

Hackers on Steroids by Oisín Sweeney
Larkspur Dreams by Anita Higman, Janice Hanna
Love Knows No Bounds by Brux, Boone, Moss, Brooke, Croft, Nina
Constable & Toop by Gareth P. Jones
Build a Man by Talli Roland
Identical by Scott Turow