The Anatomy of Violence (64 page)

Read The Anatomy of Violence Online

Authors: Adrian Raine

BOOK: The Anatomy of Violence
4.7Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Neurocriminology is now providing the foundations upon which to not just dissect the future Hannibal
Lecters and
Donta Pages, but to potentially prevent their very occurrence in the first place—if we act early. In the wake of the Newtown shootings, many officials and citizens were quick to point not only to guns as the culprit but to our general lack of mental health services. Can we do more for those all too often underserved children like Donta Page and prevent future disasters? After all, what’s so heinous about investing resources in better pre- and postnatal nutrition and care for the underserved, better elementary
school nutrition, reducing lead exposure, implementing education on parenting skills, and identifying children with serious behavior problems for benign interventions? Investing resources—costly though it may be—in the next generation of adolescents at risk for violence is not just a place on that slope where I am prepared to stand, but it’s where I hope you’ll stand with me.

Most positive societal advances somehow involve a so-called slippery slope. Can we not find ways to collectively and humanely move forward to reduce violence? Neurocriminology and a more profound understanding of the early biological causes of violence can help us take a more empathic, understanding, and merciful approach not just to the victims of violence but also to the prisoners themselves. In that process, would not the standing of all of us in an allegedly civilized society be raised?

As I sit writing here in a room in Churchill College, Cambridge, reflecting on our outlook on prisoners, my mind inevitably turns to
Winston Churchill, who himself had been a prisoner during the
Boer War. More than a hundred years ago, Churchill, as home secretary, stood up in the House of Commons and gave his
perspective on how we should treat criminals:

The mood and temper of the public in regard to the treatment of crime and criminals is one of the most unfailing tests of the civilization of any country. A calm and dispassionate recognition of the rights of the accused against the state, and even those of convicted criminals against the state, a constant heart-searching by all charged with the duty of punishment, a desire and eagerness to rehabilitate in the world of industry all those who have paid their dues in the hard coinage of punishment, tireless efforts towards the discovery of curative and regenerating processes, and an unfaltering faith that there is a treasure, if you can only find it, in the heart of every man—these are the symbols which in the treatment of crime and criminals mark and measure the stored-up strength of a nation and are the sign and proof of the living virtue in it.
83

That was more than a century ago, and yet today how calm and dispassionate have the most civilized countries in the world become on this issue? Are we tirelessly pursuing curative and regenerative
processes for the cancer of crime? Do we genuinely desire rehabilitation? Or does our mood and temper move us in anger to the costly coinage of retribution that we saw served out to
Kip Kinkel, and societal protection above all cost? How would Churchill view us today if he could see where we currently stand in our treatment of prisoners?

We look back 200 years and are aghast at an age when
mentally ill patients were kept locked in fetters and chains, and treated little better than animals because of their unacceptable behavior. In a society that was in its time at the pinnacle of world knowledge, such treatment of patients seemed totally appropriate. It was a radical and revolutionary approach for the physician
Philippe Pinel to free mentally ill patients from their shackles in Paris in 1793 and place them under more humane conditions. Today the inhumane treatment of the mentally ill seems unconscionable to us. The critical question for us to consider is whether less than a hundred years from now, a much more advanced society than the one we live in will look back aghast at our current conceptualization of violence and our concomitant incarceration and execution of prisoners with the same incredulity with which today we look back at the earlier treatment of mental patients. They may well wonder how society could have countenanced such practices and overlooked the glittering gems—small though they may be—in each and every offender who had the potential to contribute positively to society.

In a wider context, others concur with Churchill’s early vision of the potential for living virtue in our society. As
Steven Pinker eloquently outlined in his book
The Better Angels of Our Nature
, our society is moving us to be more empathic, better able to control our impulses, and to reason rather than react. The result, he argues, is that over the course of history, despite periodic swings, violence has slowly
declined.
84
The history of the world has also shown that as society becomes more ennobled and sophisticated, physical and mental disabilities such as
epilepsy,
psychosis, mental deficiency, and
alcoholism cease to be viewed in a moral or theological context and become perceived more in the humanitarian context of treatment.
85
Just as mental disorders were once viewed as a product of
evil forces, will the evil behavior of violent offenders eventually be reformulated as treatable clinical disorders? Society may deny this perspective in the short term, but I believe that a future generation with a calmer and more dispassionate perspective will indeed take this conceptual leap.

Extreme views certainly require due caution, but we must not forget
that extreme views can be appropriate, and that moderate views can be erroneous. During the
witchcraft hunts of the Reformation era in Europe, a moderate view would have been to wake up one morning and decide not to burn too many witches that day. An extreme view would have been to wake up and decide not to burn
any
witches. The notion of recidivistic violence as a clinical disorder may currently seem ludicrous to you. We must, however, face the possibility that if we close the door to even considering this perspective, we open the gates to tragedy—that breakthrough advances in remediation and treatment of
crime will be foreclosed or hopelessly stalled, and
future lives will be lost. Some think the issues are too hot to handle, or, as one leading criminologist in good faith once confided to me, “No good can ever come of genetic research on violence.” We must indeed be ever mindful of how neurobiological research findings are interpreted, as such research can be misused. Yet if we don’t allow ourselves the opportunity to consider new approaches for a better society, are we not all diminished by our blindness?

We live today in the most scientific and intellectually advanced society in the history of the world. We aspire for heavenly knowledge and have formulated firm convictions that we hold to be true. History has shown, though, that societies at different stages in history with a similar thirst for science have made grievous misjudgments under the banner of absolute knowledge. We have to cure ourselves of that irritating itch for absolute knowledge and certainty. I must consider the possibility that I err in creating a bridge between crime and
cancer. Violence may not be a clinical disorder. I do not have the answers on some issues; I am not even sure where I stand on others. Some of my scientific views are tinged with personal perspectives, and like all scientists I stand on the edge of error in my empirical research. In the same spirit of humility, I hope that in your own mind and heart you can at least consider this new zeitgeist.

What is the main message I want to leave you with? I want to suggest that society’s willingness to firmly grasp the neuroethical nettles that entangle neurocriminology, and to sensibly and cautiously integrate innovative clinical neuroscience findings with
public policy, will be a critical ingredient for our future success in violence prevention. Building further on a public health approach to violence truly has the capacity to create a healthier future. We can seize the day, change tomorrow, and create a safer world for the next generation. An open
and honest dialogue on the issues raised here will prepare the public for future developments—whatever they may be—and help facilitate future success in violence prevention.

When we finally get to 2034, will it be
utopia or dystopia? You may think that the future landscape I have painted has an
Orwellian echo—but it need not have a bleak Orwellian ending. You may recall the chant from Orwell’s
Animal Farm
of “Four legs good, two legs better,” as the privileged pigs tottered around their underling animals on two trotters. Their propaganda had closed down the minds of their comrades and created a class-based, inequitable society. Winston
Smith in the end of
1984
was reduced to doublethink—believing in two contradictory views. Perhaps the government’s
LOMBROSO program, which would tell us we can protect society and rehabilitate offenders at the same time is a similar contradictory double message. Yet if we retain an open dialogue on these issues we can prise apart doublethink and both keep our cake and eat it too.

I do believe that in tomorrow’s world we can rise above our feelings of retribution, reach out for rehabilitation, and engage in a more humane discourse on the causes of violence. After all, while we may disagree on the finer points, I believe we can all agree on our priority of
preventing
future violence. We can have a braver new world where sunshine replaces shadows. You can either stay where you are in the dark with our retributivist perspective, as I myself have been, or you can move ahead into a new day. We do have a choice—and you can choose.

We cannot continue to maintain an uncompromising mind-set, where one perspective—social or biological—dominates the other in a stranglehold over who calls the shots in curbing violence.
Amy Gutmann and
Dennis Thompson in
The Spirit of Compromise
argue that in the
polarized political arena, all sides need to give up ground in a mutual sacrifice for sound governance, adjusting long-cherished principles for the greater good.
86
Achieving this in academic criminology is an enormous challenge, requiring traditional social scientists to reverse-thrust on their long-held beliefs and embrace the anatomy of violence—a new body of knowledge that can be suffocating to some in its sophistication. Yet standing steadfast on social principles can equally stifle progress. It is up to you the reader today to help us scientists surface for air, and with your civic perspective move us forward in reevaluating where we should stand tomorrow on the seething hotbeds of violence prevention.

In the final analysis, you may decide to stand your ground and turn a blind eye to the science this book has summarized and the societal issues I have raised. You may want to believe that a biological basis to violence does not exist, or it’s going to be explained away in some manner. Like an ostrich evading the hunter, you may decide to bury your head in the sand. But if we do not make a move and act on the anatomy of violence, I believe this cancer will continue. And you had better watch out—the ostrich may get shot.

My sincere hope is that you will not turn a blind eye to the science—I want those ostriches to be alive and well. Nevertheless, you may be completely convinced that the fundamental message of the anatomy of violence is profoundly misguided. But if you happen to be a Christian, consider the words of
Oliver Cromwell when he spoke to the Church of Scotland against its intended alliance with King Charles II:

I beseech you, in the bowels of Christ, think it possible that you might be mistaken.
87

And if you are not a Christian, I beseech you in your own bowels—or any other part of your anatomy that you choose—to consider that we all have the capacity to be wrong. In dissecting the anatomy of violence, I have that capacity—don’t you too? More important than persuasion and conviction is open discussion, laying forth scientific reality, and allowing society to judiciously choose how to act in the ensuing light. My sincere hope is that our discussion will continue in the forthcoming decades and move us all into a safer and more humane society.

Notes
PREFACE

    1.
Wolfgang, M. E. (1973). Cesare Lombroso. In H. Mannheim (ed.),
Pioneers in Criminology
, pp. 232–91. Montclair, N.J.: Patterson Smith.

    2.
Sellin, T. (1937). The Lombrosian myth in criminology.
American Journal of Sociology
42, 898–99.

    3.
Kellerman, J. (1999).
Savage Spawn
:
Reflections on Violent Children.
New York: Random House.

INTRODUCTION

    1.
I was able to buy a replica of this knife at the Bodrum marketplace and realized it was a cheap knife, probably used more for threat and defense rather than as a serious weapon. I had it on my office desk at the University of Southern California as a memento until it was stolen by an office cleaner.

    2.
Wilson, J. Q. & Herrnstein, R. (1985).
Crime and Human Nature
. New York: Simon & Schuster.

1. BASIC INSTINCTS

    1.
Horn, D. G. (2003).
The Criminal Body: Lombroso and the Anatomy of Deviance
. New York: Routledge.

    2.
Gibson, M. (2002).
Born to Crime: Cesare Lombroso and the Origins of Biological Criminology
, p. 20. Westport, Conn.: Praeger.

    3.
Wolfgang, M. E. (1973). Cesare Lombroso. In H. Mannheim (ed.),
Pioneers in Criminology
, pp. 232–91. Montclair, N.J.: Patterson Smith.

    4.
Shakespeare, W. (1914).
The Tempest
, Act IV, Scene 1. London: Oxford University Press.

    5.
Gibson,
Born to Crime.

    6.
Dawkins, R. (1976).
The Selfish Gene.
New York: Oxford University Press.

    7.
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism.
Quarterly Review of Biology
46, 35–57.

    8.
Cleckley, H. C. (1976).
The Mask of Sanity.
St. Louis: Mosby.

    9.
Hare, R. D. (2003).
The Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R)
, 2nd ed. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

  10.
Harpending, H. & Draper, P. (1988). Antisocial behavior and the other side of cultural evolution. In T. E. Moffitt and S. A. Mednick (eds.),
Biological Contributions to Crime Causation
, pp. 293–307. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

  11.
Lee, R. B. & DeVore, B. I. (1976).
Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers
. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

  12.
Murphy, Y. & Murphy, R. (1974).
Women of the Forest
. New York: Columbia University Press.

  13.
Harpending & Draper, Antisocial behavior and the other side of cultural evolution.

  14.
We should not take the parallel between the Mundurucú and psychopaths too far. The lifestyle of the male Mundurucú does not exactly parallel the Western male psychopath. Western psychopaths do not in general form long-term relationships with either sex, and do not coexist and engage in joint enterprises. In contrast, the male Mundurucú do engage in long-term interpersonal relationships with members of their own sex and engage in all-male cooperative efforts for the benefit of the whole settlement.

  15.
Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of psychopathy in criminal populations.
Personality and Individual Differences
1, 111–19.

  16.
Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population.
Science
239, 985–92.

  17.
Hare, R. D. (1993).
Without Conscience: The Disturbing World of Psychopaths Amongst Us
. New York: Guilford Press.

  18.
Woodworth, M. & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides as a function of psychopathy.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
111, 436–45.

  19.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (2002).
WISQARS Leading Causes of Death Reports, 1999–2007
,
http://webapp.cdc.gov/sasweb/ncipc/leadcaus10.html
.

  20.
Overpeck, M. D., Brenner, R. A., Trumble, A. C., Trifiletti, L. B. & Berendes, H. W. (1998). Risk factors for infant homicide in the United States.
New England Journal of Medicine
339, 1211–16. While the first year of life is the time when you are most likely to be killed, for some ethnic groups this is rivaled by the risk of being a victim of homicide during adolescence and early adulthood.

  21.
Ibid.

  22.
Ibid.

  23.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1988). Evolutionary social psychology and family homicide.
Science
242, 519–24.

  24.
Wadsworth, J., Burnell, I., Taylor, B. & Butler, N. (1983). Family type and accidents in preschool-children.
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health
37, 100–104.

  25.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1988).
Homicide
. Hawthorne, N.Y.: Aldine de Gruyter.

  26.
Lightcap, J. L., Kurland, J. A. & Burgess, R. L. (1982). Child-abuse—A test of some predictions from evolutionary-theory.
Ethology and Sociobiology
3, 61–67.

  27.
Daly & Wilson, Evolutionary social psychology and family homicide.

  28.
Ibid.

  29.
Ibid.

  30.
Gottschall, J. A. & Gottschall, T. A. (2003). Are per-incident rape-pregnancy rates higher than per-incident consensual pregnancy rates?
Human Nature
14, 1–20.

  31.
Thornhill, R. & Palmer, C. (2000).
A Natural History of Rape.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

  32.
Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B. & Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for waist-hip ratio and women’s attractiveness.
Evolution and Human Behavior
31, 176–81.

  33.
Ward, T., Gannon, T. A. & Keown, K. (2006). Beliefs, values, and action: The judgment model of cognitive distortions in sexual offenders.
Aggression and Violent Behavior
11, 323–40.

  34.
Levin, R. J. & van Berlo, W. (2004). Sexual arousal and orgasm in subjects who experience forced or non-consensual sexual stimulation: A review.
Journal of Clinical Forensic Medicine
11, 82–88.

  35.
For counterarguments to the notion that orgasm can facilitate fertility and has an evolutionary basis, see Lloyd, A. E. (2005).
The Case of the Female Orgasm: Bias in the Science of Evolution
. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

  36.
Polaschek, D.L.L., Ward, T. & Hudson, S. M. (1997). Rape and rapists: Theory and treatment.
Clinical Psychology Review
17, 117–44.

  37.
McKibbin, W. F., Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T. & Starratt, V. G. (2008). Why do men rape? An evolutionary psychological perspective.
Review of General Psychology
12, 86–97.

  38.
Thornhill, N. W. & Thornhill, R. (1990). An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following rape, vol. 1, The effects of victim’s age and marital status.
Ethology and Sociobiology
11, 155–76.

  39.
Russell, D.E.H. (1990).
Rape in Marriage
. Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.

  40.
Buss, D. M. (2000).
The Dangerous Passion: Why Jealousy Is as Necessary as Love and Sex
. New York: Free Press.

  41.
Daly & Wilson. Evolutionary social psychology and family homicide.

  42.
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., Choe, J. C., Lim, H. K., et al. (1999). Jealousy and the nature of beliefs about infidelity: Tests of competing hypotheses about sex differences in the United States, Korea, and Japan.
Personal Relationships
6, 125–50.

  43.
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, G. F., Haselton, M. G., Thornhill, R., et al. (2008). Sex differences in detecting sexual infidelity: Results of a maximum likelihood method for analyzing the sensitivity of sex differences to underreporting.
Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective
19, 347–73.

  44.
Goetz, A. T. & Causey, K. (2009). Sex differences in perceptions of infidelity: Men often assume the worst.
Evolutionary Psychology
7, 253–63.

  45.
Gage, A. J. & Hutchinson, P. L. (2006). Power, control, and intimate partner sexual violence in Haiti.
Archives of Sexual Behavior
35, 11–24.

  46.
Lalumiere, M. L., Harris, G. T., Quinsey, V. L. & Rice, M. E. (2005).
The Causes of Rape: Understanding Individual Differences in Male Propensity for Sexual Aggression.
Washington, D.C.: APA Press.

  47.
Baker, R. (1996).
Sperm Wars
. New York: Basic Books.

  48.
Buss, D. M. (2009). The multiple adaptive problems solved by human aggression.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
32, 271–72.

  49.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1990). Killing the competition: Female/female and male/male homicide.
Human Nature
1, 81–107.

  50.
Wilson, M. & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk-taking, and violence: The young male syndrome.
Ethology and Sociobiology
6, 59–73.

  51.
Buss, D. M. & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Human aggression in evolutionary psychological perspective.
Clinical Psychology Review
17, 605–19.

  52.
Tremblay, R. E., Japel, C., Perusse, D., McDuff, P., Bolvin, M., et al. (1999). The search for the age of onset of physical aggression: Rousseau and Bandura revisited.
Criminal Behavior and Mental Health
9, 8–23.

  53.
Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
32, 249–311.

  54.
Ibid.

  55.
Bettencourt, B. A. & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin
119, 422–47.

  56.
Campbell, A. (1995). A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression.
Ethology and Sociobiology
16, 99–123.

  57.
Zuckerman, M. (1994).
Behavioural Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking.
New York: Cambridge University Press.

  58.
Campbell, A few good men.

  59.
Ibid.

  60.
Archer, Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?

  61.
Buss, D. N. & Dedden, L. A. (1990). Derogation of competitors.
Journal of Personality and Social Relationships
7, 395–422.

  62.
Ibid.

Other books

Enemy Mine by Karin Harlow
After Hours by Dara Girard
LeftInTheDarkness by Stephani Hecht
Magic Parcel by Frank English
Surrender by Amanda Quick
Finding the Perfect Man by Marie Higgins